Skip to main content

Contributions of early-life cognitive reserve and late-life leisure activity to successful and pathological cognitive aging

Abstract

Background

The identification of factors that specifically influence pathological and successful cognitive aging is a prerequisite for implementing disease prevention and promoting successful aging. However, multi-domain behavioral factors that characterize the difference between successful and pathological cognitive aging are not clear yet.

Methods

A group of community-dwelling older adults (N = 1347, aged 70-88 years) in Beijing was recruited in this cross-sectional study, and a sub-cohort was further divided into successful cognitive aging (SCA, N = 154), mild cognitive impairment (MCI, N = 256), and cognitively normal control (CNC, N = 173) groups. Analyses of variance, regression models with the Shapley value algorithm, and structural equation model (SEM) analyses were conducted to determine specific influencing factors and to evaluate their relative importance and interacting relationships in altering cognitive performance.

Results

We found that abundant early-life cognitive reserve (ECR, including the level of education and occupational attainment) and reduced late-life leisure activity (LLA, including mental, physical, and social activities) were distinct characteristics of SCA and MCI, respectively. The level of education, age, mental activity, and occupational attainment were the top four important factors that explained 31.6% of cognitive variability. By SEM analyses, we firstly found that LLA partially mediated the relationship between ECR and cognition; and further multi-group SEM analyses showed ECR played a more direct role in the SCA group than in the MCI group: in the SCA group, only the direct effect of ECR on cognition was significant, and in the MCI group, direct effects between ECR, LLA and cognition were all significant.

Conclusions

Results of this large-sample community-based study suggest it is important for older adults to have an abundant ECR for SCA, and to keep a high level of LLA to prevent cognitive impairment. This study clarifies the important rankings of behavioral characteristics of cognitive aging, and the relationship that ECR has a long-lasting effect on LLA and finally on cognition, providing efficient guidance for older adults to improve their cognitive function and new evidence to explain the heterogeneity of cognitive aging.

Peer Review reports

Background

The way we age is mainly decided by the quality of early-life development, the degree of middle-life maintenance, and the pace of late-life degeneration; therefore, aging heterogeneity is substantially influenced by various factors in lifespan experiences [1]. This heterogeneity is remarkably observed in cognitive aging: compared with steady cognitive changes in normal aging, accelerated cognitive decline is usually a characteristic of pathological cognitive aging in the form of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and eventually dementia [2]; on the other end of the spectrum, older adults maintaining superior cognitive performance compared with their peers are commonly referred to as exhibiting successful cognitive aging (SCA) [3]. Prior studies used various definitions of SCA, including top memory performers [4] defined using cross-sectional data, successful cognitive ager [5] and memory maintainers [6, 7] defined using longitudinal data, and superager [8, 9] defined by generational comparison (older adults vs. young or middle-aged adults). How to define SCA remains debated, but well-preserved age-sensitive cognitive functions, like episodic memory and executive function, are often-measured domains. Since pathological and successful cognitive aging are located at opposite ends of the cognitive aging spectrum, studies aiming to identify how they are influenced by multiple factors differently are very important for both disease prevention and the promotion of successful aging [10].

Factors that influence cognitive aging heterogeneity and distinguish pathological and successful cognitive aging have been reported previously, although not as extensively. In addition to advanced age, the female sex represents a strong risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease [11]. In contrast, a longitudinal study of episodic memory found that the female sex was a predictor of membership in the maintainer group [6]. High educational attainment is one of the most commonly identified variables associated with SCA [12], but studies have yielded mixed results on its effect on cognitive decline [13]. Poor physical and mental health conditions were recognized as influencing factors of pathological cognitive aging [14, 15]; the issue of whether intact health conditions positively affect SCA remains unresolved [16, 17]. The maintenance of abundant leisure activities was shown to be beneficial to cognitive function [18, 19]. In addition, multiple aspects of the geriatric lifestyle are potential influencing factors, such as diet [20] and sleep quality [21]. From the lifespan perspective, the aforementioned factors are divided into two types. Factors throughout early and middle life, including educational and occupational attainments, etc., are usually nonmodifiable for older adults. Nevertheless, these factors have close relationships with cognition as cognitive reserve proxies [22, 23]. Certainly, many late-life modifiable factors have been identified, including leisure activities and daily lifestyle, etc. in old age [24]. More systematic investigations are needed to identify the specific roles of these factors in cognitive aging heterogeneity.

Although previous studies have provided lots of reference information on the factors influencing cognitive aging, several core issues still require further investigation. First, the specific or common effects of these multiple factors on pathological and successful cognitive aging are unclear. Factors influencing pathological cognitive aging have been reported and related prevention measures have been recommended to decrease the risk of related diseases, such as dementia [24]. In contrast, factors influencing SCA are relatively unconfirmed, and the degree of overlap with factors influencing pathological cognitive aging is not sufficiently depicted [16, 25]. Second, the relative importance rankings of these multiple factors in explaining cognitive heterogeneity are still largely unknown. Finally, the aforementioned early-life and late-life factors both exert direct effects on cognition; nevertheless, it’s not clear whether the relationship between them further explains their effects on cognitive aging, especially for factors with a high rank of importance in predicting cognitive heterogeneity.

In the current study, we sought first to identify influencing factors specific or common to successful and pathological cognitive aging by comparing the multidomain characteristics of SCA, MCI (typical representation of pathological cognitive aging), and cognitively normal control (CNC) groups. We then evaluated the relative importance of the identified factors to cognitive heterogeneity. Finally, we examined the relationships between early-life factors, late-life factors, and cognitive function. We hypothesized that SCA and MCI had both overlapping and specific influencing factors, and the overlapping factors would be more important in explaining cognitive heterogeneity. We also hypothesized the existence of a mediation effect of late-life factors on the relationship between early-life factors and cognition in addition to their detectable direct effects on cognition.

Methods

Participants

All participants were from the Beijing Aging Brain Rejuvenation Initiative (BABRI), an ongoing cohort study investigating aging and cognitive impairment in China [26]. The inclusion criteria for this study were as follows: (1) aged 70 years or older, (2) a score of at least 24 on the Chinese version of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [27], (3) no history of severe neurological and psychiatric conditions, (4) no history of taking psychoactive medications, and (5) completed a battery of neuropsychological assessments. Among 7625 participants from the BABRI baseline database, 1347 non-demented older adults whose ages were larger than 70 (i.e., the full sample) were included following the criteria above in the current study, and these participants were further assigned to different groups (SCA, MCI, CNC) according to the definitions described below (shown in Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow chart of the participant inclusion process. *Total data volume of the BABRI database (n = 7625) shown in this flowchart is up to 2018.1, the data volume is increasing since then and BABRI has now recruited over 10,000 participants [26]. Abbreviation: MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; AVLT-N5, Auditory verbal learning test long-time delayed recall; TMT-B, Trail-Making Test Part B; SD, standard deviation; ADL, activities of daily living scale

Neuropsychological tests and grouping methods

Cognitive assessments covered multiple domains, including memory, attention, visuospatial ability, and executive function, and each domain was evaluated by two tests, as implemented previously [28]. The operationalized definition of SCA in this study was (1) excellent episodic memory or executive function, i.e., scoring higher than 1.5 standard deviations (SDs) above the age- and education-adjusted mean on the Auditory Verbal Learning Test long-time delayed recall (AVLT-N5) or the Trail-Making Test part B (TMT-B); and (2) intact general cognition, as evidenced the lack of a score on any test lower than 1.5 SDs below the age- and education-adjusted means. The diagnostic criteria for MCI were based on Petersen’s criteria [29]: (1) subjective cognitive complaints; (2) cognitive impairment in at least one domain, i.e., scoring lower than 1.5 SDs below the age- and education-adjusted mean on both tests in any domain and (3) the activities of daily living (ADL) [30] scored 0. The CNC group was defined as non-MCI and scored between 0.5 SDs below and 0.5 SDs above the mean on both AVLT-N5 and TMT-B. Based on the aforementioned definitions, the grouped sample (N = 583) included 154 participants with SCA (age range 70-88 years, mean age 73.84 ± 3.78 years, 100 females), 173 CNC participants (age range 70-86 years, mean age 74.80 ± 3.89 years, 88 females) and 256 participants with MCI (age range 70-88 years, mean age 76.11 ± 4.15 years, 133 females).

How to best distinguish supernormal cognitive maintenance in aging remains a controversial topic, and there are numerous operational definitions of SCA in previous studies [25]. In the current study, we paid attention to older adults whose ages were equal to or above 70, by which older adults have experienced the main process of age-related cognitive decline according to cognitive aging trajectory studies [31]; and those who still maintain superior cognitive performance at age 70 and above are more likely to be members of the SCA group than those under the age of 70. Regarding cognitive domains, episodic memory and executive function are given priority in the current study while defining SCA. Episodic memory and executive function are both sensitive to age-related decline [32] and maintaining superior performance in these two domains implies little age-related cognitive decline. The rationality of using these measures (AVLT-N5 and TMT-B for episodic memory and executive function, separately) is also recognized by prior work [5, 33], Moreover, no impaired domain was allowed in our SCA definition.

Collection of behavioral characteristics

Detailed demographic information, mental and physical health conditions, living habits, and leisure activities were collected in one-to-one interviews between trained testers and participants. Demographic information included age, gender, level of education, occupational attainment, marriage status, and income level. The level of education was confirmed as years of formal schooling at an early age; and occupational attainment was measured by the occupational prestige scale (range 0-1) which represents the degree of occupational prestige and social status [34, 35]. Marriage status was divided into two types, married and single (for various reasons, including unmarried, widowed, and divorced); the income level was scored from one to thirteen, and a higher score indicated more monthly income for the participant. Surveys on mental health conditions included life self-satisfaction, memory self-satisfaction, loneliness, and depression. Life self-satisfaction was measured using the face scale, which included seven stick figures of faces that expressed increasing degrees of happiness from “extremely sad” to “extremely happy”; memory self-satisfaction was measured using the hill scale, in which older adults selected a step from 11 steps to represent their degree of satisfaction with their current memory function; Loneliness and depression were tested using the UCLA loneliness scale [36] and the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) [37], respectively. Physical health conditions were measured both subjectively and objectively. The subjective health status was reported by participants using a four-point rating scale (“good”, “normal”, “bad” or “poor”), and objective measures were collected including body mass index (BMI) and histories of multiple diseases, including diabetes, hypertension, hyperlipidemia, cerebrovascular disease, and coronary heart disease (all were marked as “0” for healthy individuals and “1” for patients). Living habits included diet, sleep quality, and histories of smoking and drinking. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) scale [38] was used. Finally, leisure activity was measured using a scale with 23 items that were recognized as common activities in the older Chinese population. We used the weighted grade method to calculate scores for mental, physical, and social activities, as implemented previously [28, 39, 40].

Statistical analysis

A three-step procedure was conducted as follows. First, we evaluated intergroup differences (SCA, CNC, MCI) in cognitive performance and behavioral characteristics. ANOVA, χ2, and Kruskal-Wallis tests were performed for continuous, categorical, and ordinal dependent variables, respectively, using SPSS 24.0 (IBM Corp, Armonk, NY), and factors with significant intergroup differences were identified. Secondly, we examined the significance of these identified factors in predicting cognitive performance (calculated as the average Z-score of each cognitive domain) using multivariable regression analyses. We then determined the relative importance of each factor by calculating the “lmg” metric using the “relaimpo” package [41] of R (version 4.0.3) to decompose the explained R2 of the regression model into nonnegative contributions of factors that automatically sum to the total R2 in the form of percentages. The Shapley value algorithm [42] was used to obtain the relative importance of each factor (see the equations below for the “lmg” metric, which is the average increase in R2 attributed to each factor).

$$\textrm{seq}{R}^2\left(\left\{{\textrm{x}}_{\textrm{k}}\right\}|{\textrm{S}}_{\textrm{k}}\left(\textrm{r}\right)\right)={R}^2\left(\left\{{\textrm{x}}_{\textrm{k}}\right\}\cup {\textrm{S}}_{\textrm{k}}\left(\textrm{r}\right)\right)-{R}^2\left({\textrm{S}}_{\textrm{k}}\left(\textrm{r}\right)\right)$$
(1)
$$\textrm{LMG}\left({\textrm{x}}_{\textrm{k}}\right)=\frac{1}{\textrm{p}!}\sum_{{\textrm{S}}_{\textrm{k}}\subseteq \left\{{\textrm{x}}_1,\dots, {\textrm{x}}_{\textrm{p}}\right\}\setminus \left\{{\textrm{x}}_{\textrm{k}}\right\}}\textrm{n}\left({\textrm{S}}_{\textrm{k}}\left(\textrm{r}\right)\right)!\left(\textrm{p}-\textrm{n}\left({\textrm{S}}_{\textrm{k}}\left(\textrm{r}\right)\right)-1\right)!\textrm{seq}{R}^2\left(\left\{{\textrm{x}}_{\textrm{k}}\right\}|{\textrm{S}}_{\textrm{k}}\left(\textrm{r}\right)\right)$$
(2)

The order of regressors in the regression model was determined based on all possible permutations of the available regressors (i.e., identified influencing factors) x1 … xp, and the order was denoted by the tuple of indices r = (r1 … rp). The denominator p! was the number of different permutations of all regressors, Sk denoted the set of regressors extracted from all regressors except for xk, Sk(r) denoted the set of regressors entered into the model before the regressor xk in the order r, and the number of permutations when the regressor xk in the order r was counted as n(Sk(r)) ! (p − n(Sk(r)) − 1)!. The increase in R2 when adding xk to the model with the regressors in set Sk(r) was calculated as seqR2({xk}| Sk(r)) in Eq. (1). In this study, p = 9.

In the third step, the SEM approach was used to test the proposed hypotheses of the relationships among early-life factors, late-life factors, and cognitive performance. The observed variables were first standardized and then put into the model. The root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), comparative fit index (CFI), and Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) were used to assess the goodness of fit in SEM [43]. In addition, we examined the significance of factor loadings, path coefficients (β), and mediating effects using a bias-corrected bootstrap 95% confidence interval (CI) generated after a bootstrap analysis that resampled the data 1000 times. We first conducted SEM analyses with the grouped sample (N = 583) and supplementary analyses with the full sample (N = 1347) and then built models of the three groups separately to determine any differences. SEM analyses were conducted using Mplus V.8.3 software (www.statmodel.com).

Results

Multifactorial between-group differences

Between-group differences in cognitive performance and behavioral characteristics are summarized in Table 1 and Table S1. Sequential cognitive performances in all domains were observed in the order of best performance in SCA and worst in MCI, with CNC in between. The SCA group was younger than the CNC group, and both groups were younger than the MCI group. The SCA group had a larger proportion of women, higher levels of educational and occupational attainment, higher levels of memory self-satisfaction, and was more frequently involved in mental activities than the other two groups. In addition, the SCA group had a lower BMI than the CNC group. There was no significant difference between CNC and MCI in gender, level of education, occupational attainment, and BMI. The MCI group had worse memory self-satisfaction and participated in leisure activities less frequently than the other two groups, and there was no significant difference between SCA and CNC in physical and social activities. Taken together, we found that age, gender, level of education, occupational attainment, BMI, memory self-satisfaction, mental activity, physical activity, and social activity showed significant intergroup differences, and further analyses were conducted on these nine factors. Age, memory self-satisfaction, and mental activity were overlapping influencing factors of SCA and MCI. The results of specific items in the leisure activity scale are presented in Table S2.

Table 1 Significant intergroup differences in multidomain cognitive performance and behavioral characteristics of the three groups

Relative importance rank of the identified factors

The multivariable linear regression analysis of the grouped sample revealed that 36.0% of the total variance in cognitive performance was explained by these nine identified factors. Relative importance analyses found that the level of education (explaining 43.9% of the total R2) was the most important factor, followed by age (20.6%), mental activity (12.1%), and occupational attainment (11.3%) (shown in Table 2). These top four factors explained 31.6% of the total cognitive variance. Four cognitive domains were explained by these nine identified factors in different proportions: the R2 was 0.289 for memory, 0.158 for visuospatial ability, 0.266 for attention, and 0.167 for executive function. The level of education, age, mental activity, and occupational attainment were the top four factors for all cognitive domains, except that memory self-satisfaction attained the fourth position in the memory domain. The importance rankings of these factors were similar when analyzing the full sample (shown in Table S3).

Table 2 Results of the relative importance of influencing factors to each cognitive domain in the grouped sample (N = 583)

Assessment of relationships among early- and late-life factors and cognition

In our hypothesized model, the independent variable was a latent one (named early-life cognitive reserve, ECR) generated by educational and occupational attainment, and dependent variables were multidomain cognitive abilities including memory, visuospatial ability, attention, and executive function; moreover, a latent variable named as late-life leisure activity (LLA) was generated by mental, physical and social activities and was designed as the underlying mediating variable. In the grouped sample, the model fit indices were RMSEA = 0.062, CFI = 0.964, and TLI = 0.947, all of which indicated a good fit. Figure 2A shows the model of the grouped sample with standardized path coefficients, and a summary of all metrics of this model is provided in Table S4. LLA partially mediated the effects of ECR on memory, attention, and executive function, with indirect effects of 0.055, 0.063, and 0.059, corresponding to p = 0.002, 0.001, and 0.001, respectively. The model of the grouped sample with general cognitive function as dependent variable (latent variable generated by Z-scores of the four domains) also fitted well (RMSEA = 0.076, CFI = 0.956, and TLI = 0.933), and the partial mediating effect of LLA was significant (estimated mediating effect was 0.078, p < 0.001, shown in Fig. 2B and Table S5). The aforementioned models and results were also validated in the full sample (N = 1347), please see Fig. S1, Tables S6 and S7.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Structural equation models that reveal relationships among early-life cognitive reserve, late-life leisure activity, and cognitive performance. Full lines with arrows indicate significant paths and dotted lines indicate insignificant paths. Bold numbers without asterisks are significant path coefficients (β, all p < 0.001), bold numbers with asterisks (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, and ***p < 0.001) indicate indirect effects, and narrow numbers are residual variances. Metrics that represent the goodness of model fit are listed separately. A Grouped sample (N = 583) SEM model built with early-life cognitive reserve, late-life leisure activity, and four cognitive domains. B Grouped sample (N = 583) SEM model built with early-life cognitive reserve, late-life leisure activity, and general cognitive function. Abbreviation: ECR, early-life cognitive reserve; LLA, late-life leisure activity; MEM, memory; VSA, visuospatial ability; ATT, attention; EF, executive function; EDU, level of education; OCC, occupational attainment; MA, mental activity; PA, physical activity; SA, social activity; N5, auditory verbal learning test long-time delayed recall; N1N5, auditory verbal learning test total recall; CFT, Rey-osterrieth complex figure test; CDT, clock-drawing test; SDMT, symbol digit modalities test; TMTA, trail-making test part A; SCWT, stroop color-word test; TMTB, trail-making test part B; COG, general cognitive function; Z-MEM, mean Z-score of tests in memory domain; Z-VSA, mean Z-score of tests in visuospatial ability domain; Z-ATT, mean Z-score of tests in attention domain; Z-EF, mean Z-score of tests in executive function domain

SEM models were further built for each of the three groups separately, with general cognitive function as the dependent variable. In the SCA group, the model fit indices were RMSEA = 0.060, CFI = 0.972, and TLI = 0.953, and the path from ECR to cognition was the only significant path in this model (shown in Fig. 3A and Table S8). In the CNC group, the model also displayed a good fit (RMSEA = 0.073, CFI = 0.966, and TLI = 0.944), and paths from ECR to both LLA and cognition were significant (shown in Fig. 3B and Table S8). In the MCI group, three paths between ECR, LLA, and general cognitive function were all significant, although this model fit less satisfactorily (RMSEA = 0.097, CFI = 0.922, and TLI = 0.871) (shown in Fig. 3C and Table S8). The mediating effect of LLA was insignificant in all three group-specific models listed above (SCA model, standardized β = 0.022, p = 0.440; CNC model, standardized β = − 0.057, p = 0.564; MCI model, standardized β = 0.073, p = 0.096). A tendency of differences in the effects of ECR and LLA on cognition in SCA and MCI groups was found: ECR, rather than LLA, exerted a pivotal and direct effect on the superior cognitive performance of the SCA group; but for individuals with MCI, both LLA and ECR played considerable roles on determining their cognition.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Intergroup trend of differences in structural equation models for different groups. Full lines indicate significant paths and dotted lines indicate insignificant paths. Bold numbers without asterisks are significant path coefficients (β, all p < 0.05), bold numbers with asterisks (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, and ***p < 0.001) indicate indirect effects, and narrow numbers are residual variances. Metrics that represent the goodness of model fit are listed separately. A SEM model of the SCA group (N = 154); B SEM model of the CNC group (N = 173); C SEM model of the MCI group (N = 256). Abbreviation: ECR, early-life cognitive reserve; LLA, late-life leisure activity; COG, general cognitive function; EDU, level of education; OCC, occupational attainment; MA, mental activity; PA, physical activity; SA, social activity; Z-MEM, mean Z-score of tests in memory domain; Z-VSA, mean Z-score of tests in visuospatial ability domain; Z-ATT, mean Z-score of tests in attention domain; Z-EF, mean Z-score of tests in executive function domain

Discussion

The current study showed that abundant early-life cognitive reserve and reduced late-life leisure activity were distinct characteristics of SCA and MCI, respectively. Common characteristics contributing to both aging processes were age, memory self-satisfaction, and mental activity. The level of education, age, mental activity, and occupational attainment were the top four important factors explaining 31.6% of cognitive aging heterogeneity. The results of SEM analyses showed that both early-life cognitive reserve and late-life leisure activity exerted direct effects on multiple cognitive domains, and late-life leisure activity partially mediated the relationship between early-life cognitive reserve and cognitive performance.

Our results suggested that a high level of early-life cognitive reserve played a central role in SCA by identifying that early-life educational and occupational attainment were specific factors influencing SCA. These two factors ranked in the top four in terms of importance in predicting cognitive performance, and early-life cognitive reserve exerted a significant and steady direct effect on cognitive performance in SEM models built in the grouped sample, the full sample, and the SCA group. This result is consistent with a previous study showing that SCA is determined by cognitive reserve acquired throughout life and mainly developed during the years of formal education [44]. In addition to early-life cognitive reserve, female sex and low BMI were specific characteristics of SCA. Previous studies have provided evidence supporting this female superiority in the longitudinal maintenance of cognitive function [5, 6]. The potential effect of obesity on cognitive aging is still controversial [45]; the low BMI characteristic of the SCA group found in this study requires further examination. We found that the MCI group tended to be inactive in daily life, and this result supported previous studies showing that interventions upregulating older adults’ multi-domain activity exerted positive effects on preventing pathological aging [18, 46]. Furthermore, this study implied previous work might have confused the group difference by including SCA older adults in the non-pathological aging group together with CNC. For example, we found educational differences between the SCA and CNC groups, but not between the MCI and CNC groups, although the level of education showed a group difference between pathological and non-pathological aging older adults [47].

Age, mental activity and memory self-satisfaction were overlapping factors contributing to both successful and pathological cognitive aging. Advanced age is undoubtedly an obvious risk factor for cognitive decline. Memory self-satisfaction reflects older adults’ subjective cognitive evaluation, which is valuable for the early diagnosis of subjective cognitive decline (SCD) [48]. In this study, regression analyses revealed this subjective evaluation significantly predicted objective memory performance (β =0.14, p = 0.008, lmg = 9.2%, shown in Table 2). Various types of intellectual engagements serve as proxy measures of cognitive reserve [49, 50], which continues to accumulate during the lifespan and in old age by participating in mental activities and cognitive training [46]. This study identified four pivotal factors that explained 31.6% of cognitive aging heterogeneity, and among them, the level of education, occupational attainment, and mental activity were all proxies of cognitive reserve [50], or intellectual enrichment. We propose that interventions aiming to upregulate lifetime intellectual enrichment would be effective measures for promoting successful cognitive aging.

The results of SEM analyses indicated that early-life cognitive reserve had a long-lasting influence on late-life leisure activity and subsequent cognitive performance. SEM analyses of different groups further revealed that early-life cognitive reserve played a pivotal role in the SCA group. The high early-life cognitive reserve may be the driving force for the maintenance of cognitive function in old age in the SCA group, which supports the “preserved differentiation” theory of how an influencing factor may operate concerning cognitive aging [1]. Nonetheless, a longitudinal study is necessary to further confirm the effect of early-life cognitive reserve on cognitive maintenance and decline throughout the lifespan. Because of the small sample size and limited cognitive variabilities within each group, the mediating effects of late-life leisure activity were insignificant in multi-group SEM analyses; on the other hand, the identified factors played more important roles in directing the different aging paths of participants than in explaining the variation of participants’ cognitive performance within each of the three groups.

The current study has some limitations. First, MCI cannot represent all behavioral features of pathological cognitive aging processes, but only typical ones of them; and there is more variance in pathological cognitive aging individuals (for example, dementia patients) that needs further investigation. Second, a consensus on the definition of SCA has still not been reached, previous studies have used different cognitive measures to define SCA, and we selected two high-profile domains (episodic memory and executive function); however, the generalization of the results of this study to other models of SCA should be performed with caution. Third, the current study only paid attention to the nine characteristics of interest and didn’t evaluate the underlying effects of other factors on cognition, resulting that nearly two-thirds of the total variance not being explained by the current regression algorithm and potential biases may exist, future studies should further explore other factors and their relative importance. In addition, although the current study tried to clarify the relationships between early-life and late-life influencing factors using SEM analyses, only longitudinal data will be capable of revealing the determinants of cognitive aging throughout life.

Conclusions

This study found that early-life cognitive reserve and late-life leisure activity were specific characteristics of successful and pathological cognitive aging, separately, and suggested that measures upregulating lifetime intellectual enrichment would be beneficial to late-life cognitive performance.

Availability of data and materials

The dataset analyzed during the current study are not publicly available as these might contain information that could compromise the privacy of research participants, and the BABRI database continues to be constructed in an ongoing cohort study. Data analyzed in the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Abbreviations

SCA:

Successful cognitive aging

MCI:

Mild cognitive impairment

CNC:

Cognitively normal control

MMSE:

Mini-mental state examination

AVLT-N5:

Auditory verbal learning test long-time delayed recall

TMT-B:

Trail-making test part B

BMI:

Body mass index

SEM:

Structural equation model

RMSEA:

Root mean square error of approximation

CFI:

Comparative fit index

TLI:

Tucker-Lewis index

ECR:

Early-life cognitive reserve

LLA:

Late-life leisure activity

References

  1. Tucker-Drob EM. Cognitive aging and dementia: a life-span perspective. Annu Rev Dev Psychol. 2019;1(1):177–96.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Sperling RA, Aisen PS, Beckett LA, Bennett DA, Craft S, Fagan AM, et al. Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):280–92.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Depp CA, Harmell A, Vahia IV. Successful cognitive aging. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2012;10:35–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Dekhtyar M, Papp KV, Buckley R, Jacobs HIL, Schultz AP, Johnson KA, et al. Neuroimaging markers associated with maintenance of optimal memory performance in late-life. Neuropsychologia. 2017;100:164–70.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Lin FV, Wang X, Wu R, Rebok GW, Chapman BP, Initiative AsDN. Identification of successful cognitive aging in the Alzheimer’s disease neuroimaging initiative study. J Alzheimer's Dis. 2017;59(1):101–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Josefsson M, de Luna X, Pudas S, Nilsson LG, Nyberg L. Genetic and lifestyle predictors of 15-year longitudinal change in episodic memory. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2012;60(12):2308–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Nyberg L, Boraxbekk CJ, Sorman DE, Hansson P, Herlitz A, Kauppi K, et al. Biological and environmental predictors of heterogeneity in neurocognitive ageing: evidence from Betula and other longitudinal studies. Ageing Res Rev. 2020;64:101184.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Harrison TM, Weintraub S, Mesulam MM, Rogalski E. Superior memory and higher cortical volumes in unusually successful cognitive aging. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2012;18(6):1081–5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Sun FW, Stepanovic MR, Andreano J, Barrett LF, Touroutoglou A, Dickerson BC. Youthful brains in older adults: preserved neuroanatomy in the default mode and salience networks contributes to youthful memory in Superaging. J Neurosci. 2016;36(37):9659–68.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Rosenberg A, Ngandu T, Rusanen M, Antikainen R, Backman L, Havulinna S, et al. Multidomain lifestyle intervention benefits a large elderly population at risk for cognitive decline and dementia regardless of baseline characteristics: the FINGER trial. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14(3):263–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Podcasy JL, Epperson CN. Considering sex and gender in Alzheimer disease and other dementias. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2016;18(4):437–46.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Yaffe K, Fiocco AJ, Lindquist K, Vittinghoff E, Simonsick EM, Newman AB, et al. Predictors of maintaining cognitive function in older adults: the health ABC study. Neurology. 2009;72(23):2029–35.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Seblova D, Berggren R, Lövdén M. Education and age-related decline in cognitive performance: systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies. Ageing Res Rev. 2020;58:101005.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Samieri C, Perier MC, Gaye B, Proust-Lima C, Helmer C, Dartigues JF, et al. Association of cardiovascular health level in older age with cognitive decline and incident dementia. JAMA. 2018;320(7):657–64.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Wang S, Blazer DG. Depression and cognition in the elderly. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2015;11:331–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Anton SD, Woods AJ, Ashizawa T, Barb D, Buford TW, Carter CS, et al. Successful aging: advancing the science of physical independence in older adults. Ageing Res Rev. 2015;24(Pt B):304–27.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Hittner EF, Stephens JE, Turiano NA, Gerstorf D, Lachman ME, Haase CM. Positive affect is associated with less memory decline: evidence from a 9-year longitudinal study. Psychol Sci. 2020;31(11):1386–95.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Karssemeijer EGA, Aaronson JA, Bossers WJ, Smits T, Olde Rikkert MGM, Kessels RPC. Positive effects of combined cognitive and physical exercise training on cognitive function in older adults with mild cognitive impairment or dementia: a meta-analysis. Ageing Res Rev. 2017;40:75–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Dause TJ, Kirby ED. Aging gracefully: social engagement joins exercise and enrichment as a key lifestyle factor in resistance to age-related cognitive decline. Neural Regen Res. 2019;14(1):39–42.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Scarmeas N, Anastasiou CA, Yannakoulia M. Nutrition and prevention of cognitive impairment. Lancet Neurol. 2018;17(11):1006–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Mander BA, Winer JR, Walker MP. Sleep and human aging. Neuron. 2017;94(1):19–36.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Kremen WS, Beck A, Elman JA, Gustavson DE, Reynolds CA, Tu XM, et al. Influence of young adult cognitive ability and additional education on later-life cognition. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2019;116(6):2021–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Stern Y, Barnes CA, Grady C, Jones RN, Raz N. Brain reserve, cognitive reserve, compensation, and maintenance: operationalization, validity, and mechanisms of cognitive resilience. Neurobiol Aging. 2019;83:124–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the lancet commission. Lancet. 2020;396(10248):413–46.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Nyberg L, Pudas S. Successful memory aging. Annu Rev Psychol. 2019;70:219–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Yang C, Li X, Zhang J, Chen Y, Li H, Wei D, et al. Early prevention of cognitive impairment in the community population: the Beijing aging brain rejuvenation initiative. Alzheimers Dement. 2021;17(10):1610–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Zhang MY, Katzman R, Salmon D, Jin H, Cai GJ, Wang ZY, et al. The prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease in Shanghai, China: impact of age, gender, and education. Ann Neurol. 1990;27(4):428–37.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Li X, Ma C, Zhang J, Liang Y, Chen Y, Chen K, et al. Prevalence of and potential risk factors for mild cognitive impairment in community-dwelling residents of Beijing. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2013;61(12):2111–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Petersen RC, Morris JC. Mild cognitive impairment as a clinical entity and treatment target. Arch Neurol-Chicago. 2005;62(7):1160–3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Lawton MP, Brody EM. Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist. 1969;9(3):179–86.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Li H, Lv C, Zhang T, Chen K, Chen C, Gai G, et al. Trajectories of age-related cognitive decline and potential associated factors of cognitive function in senior citizens of Beijing. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2014;11(8):806–16.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Park DC, Reuter-Lorenz P. The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009;60:173–96.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Katsumi Y, Andreano JM, Barrett LF, Dickerson BC, Touroutoglou A. Greater neural differentiation in the ventral visual cortex is associated with youthful memory in Superaging. Cereb Cortex. 2021;31(11):5275–87.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Treiman DJ. A standard occupational prestige scale for use with historical data. J Interdiscip Hist. 1976;7(2):283–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Li C. Cleavage and fragment: an empirical analysis on the social stratification of the contemporary China. China: Social Sciences Academic Press; 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Russell DW. UCLA loneliness scale (version 3): reliability, validity, and factor structure. J Pers Assess. 1996;66(1):20–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Brink TL, Yesavage JA, Lum O, Heersema PH, Adey M, Rose TL. Screening tests for geriatric depression. Clin Gerontol. 1982;1(1):37–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28(2):193–213.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Karp A, Paillard-Borg S, Wang HX, Silverstein M, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L. Mental, physical and social components in leisure activities equally contribute to decrease dementia risk. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2006;21(2):65–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Chen Y, Lv C, Li X, Zhang J, Chen K, Liu Z, et al. The positive impacts of early-life education on cognition, leisure activity, and brain structure in healthy aging. Aging (Albany NY). 2019;11(14):4923–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Groemping U. Relative importance for linear regression in R: the package relaimpo. J Stat Softw. 2006;17(1):925–33.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Shapley LS. A value for n-person games. In: Kuhn HW, Tucker AW, editors. Contributions to the theory of games II, annals of mathematics studies. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 1953. p. 307–17.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model Multidiscip J. 1999;6(1):1–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Balduino E, de Melo BAR, de Sousa Mota da Silva L, Martinelli JE, Cecato JF. The “SuperAgers” construct in clinical practice: neuropsychological assessment of illiterate and educated elderly. Int Psychogeriatr. 2020;32(2):191–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Dye L, Boyle NB, Champ C, Lawton C. The relationship between obesity and cognitive health and decline. Proc Nutr Soc. 2017;76(4):443–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Hill NT, Mowszowski L, Naismith SL, Chadwick VL, Valenzuela M, Lampit A. Computerized cognitive training in older adults with mild cognitive impairment or dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2017;174(4):329–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Baumgart M, Snyder HM, Carrillo MC, Fazio S, Kim H, Johns H. Summary of the evidence on modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline and dementia: a population-based perspective. Alzheimers Dement. 2015;11(6):718–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Rabin LA, Smart CM, Amariglio RE. Subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2017;13:369–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Stern Y. Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 2012;11(11):1006–12.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Cabeza R, Albert M, Belleville S, Craik FIM, Duarte A, Grady CL, et al. Maintenance, reserve and compensation: the cognitive neuroscience of healthy ageing. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2018;19(11):701–10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank all the volunteers for their participation and collaboration in this study.

Funding

This work was supported by Science and Technology Innovation 2030 Major Projects (grant number 2022ZD0211600), State Key Program of National Natural Science of China (grant number 82130118), Funds for International Cooperation and Exchange of the National Natural Science Foundation of China (grant number 81820108034), National Science Fund for Distinguished Young Scholars (grant number 81625025), and the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (grant number 2017XTCX04).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Y. Yang and Z. Zhang developed the study concept. All authors contributed to the study design. Data collection was performed by X. Li, Y. Chen, C. Yang, Y. Yang, and other members of the BABRI group. Y. Yang performed the data analysis and interpretation under the supervision of Z. Zhang. Y. Yang drafted the manuscript, and Y. Chen, C. Yang, and K. Chen provided critical revisions. The author(s) read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Zhanjun Zhang.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Written informed consent was obtained from each participant, and the use of human subjects was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) at the Imaging Center for Brain Research at Beijing Normal University in September 2009 for the first time. The approval is re-checked every year ever after, and the approval number is ICBIR_A_0041_002_02. Participant inclusion was conducted in October 2009 and is still ongoing now. All methods were performed following the relevant guidelines and regulations.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Table S1.

Intergroup differences in demographics, health conditions, and living habits. Table S2. Intergroup differences in detailed items of leisure activities. Table S3. Results of the relative importance of influencing factors to each cognitive domain in the full sample (N = 1347). Table S4. Estimated direct and indirect effects of the grouped sample (N = 583) SEM with ECR, LLA, and four cognitive domains as latent factors. Table S5. Estimated direct and indirect effects of the grouped sample (N = 583) SEM with ECR, LLA, and general cognitive function as latent factors. Table S6. Estimated direct and indirect effects of the full sample (N = 1347) SEM with ECR, LLA, and four cognitive domains as latent factors. Table S7. Estimated direct and indirect effects of the full sample (N = 1347) SEM with ECR, LLA, and general cognitive function as latent factors. Table S8. Estimated direct and indirect effects of the multi-group SEM with ECR, LLA, and general cognitive function as latent factors. Figure S1. Structural equation models that reveal relationships among ECR, LLA, and cognitive performance in the full sample (N = 1347).

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Yang, Y., Chen, Y., Yang, C. et al. Contributions of early-life cognitive reserve and late-life leisure activity to successful and pathological cognitive aging. BMC Geriatr 22, 831 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-022-03530-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-022-03530-5

Keywords