Skip to main content

Relationships of self-management abilities to loneliness among older people: a cross-sectional study

Abstract

Background

We investigated relationships of broader self-management abilities (self-efficacy, positive frame of mind, investment behavior, taking initiatives, multifunctionality of resources, variety of resources) to social and emotional loneliness among community-dwelling older people while controlling for background characteristics.

Methods

This cross-sectional study employed a representative sample of 41,327 community-dwelling people aged ≥55 years in Limburg, the Netherlands, identified using the population register (weighted per district, complex sampling design). In total, 20,327 (50%) people responded to the questionnaire.

Results

All self-management abilities were associated negatively with emotional loneliness. Taking initiatives, multifunctionality, self-efficacy, and a positive frame of mind were associated negatively with social loneliness. Self-efficacy had the strongest relationships with social and emotional loneliness.

Conclusions

In combatting loneliness among older people, investment in their ability to self-manage their social lives and activities, such as increasing opportunities for positive social interaction and social support and reducing maladaptive cognition, seems to be crucial.

Peer Review reports

Background

The Netherlands — along with the rest of the world — faces serious challenges as a direct consequence of populational aging. The absolute number of older people in the Netherlands is expected to increase from 2.4 million (16%) in 2010 to 4.6 million (26%) in 2040 [1, 2]. The same picture emerges across Europe, where the percentage of people aged ≥65 years increased from 13.7% in 1990 to 17.4% in 2010, and is predicted to increase even further to 30% by 2060 [3]. Loneliness is highly prevalent among older people [4,5,6]; up to one-third of older people experience feelings of loneliness [7,8,9]. Research clearly shows the detrimental impacts of loneliness on health outcomes, morbidity, mortality, and healthcare use [10,11,12,13,14,15].

Although loneliness is especially prevalent among the very old, it is not related to age alone [4, 16]. Associations between age and loneliness can be explained mainly by age-related health problems and changes in social network ties [17,18,19,20,21,22]. Societal changes in recent decades, such as the reduction of family size, increased number of people who stay single throughout their lives, increased divorce rates, greater distances between family members, and reduced number of people living in multigenerational households, have influenced the occurrence of social and emotional loneliness among older people [4, 23]. Furthermore, associations have been found between loneliness and older people’s background characteristics. Marital status, for example, is clearly associated with loneliness, with married people found to be at an advantage in this regard in many studies [17,18,19, 22, 24, 25]. Mixed findings, however, have been reported on the relationship between gender and loneliness. Although women appear to be lonelier than their male counterparts [26], this gender effect disappears after adjustment for socioeconomic and health variables [19, 27, 28]. Mixed results have also been obtained regarding the relationship between loneliness and socioeconomic status (income and education); some researchers have reported such an association [9, 21, 25], whereas others have found none [19, 29, 30].

Perlman and Peplau [31], p. 31 defined loneliness as “the unpleasant experience that occurs when a person’s network of social relationships is deficient in some important way, either quantitatively or qualitatively.” Loneliness occurs when the number of existing relationships a person has is smaller than desired (quantitative deficiency in one’s network of social relationships) or when the desired intimacy has not been realized (qualitative deficiency in one’s network of social relationships) [32, 33]. It is an expression of negative feelings about certain missing aspects in a person’s social relationships which can occur throughout the life-course [33]. The prevalence is, however, much higher among individuals of advanced age [8, 9]. Weiss [34] identified two components of loneliness: emotional loneliness (stems from the absence or loss of a close attachment relationship) and social loneliness (stems from the absence of an engaging social network). Emotional loneliness arises after life events, such as losing a loved one through death or ending of a marriage, leading to intense feelings of abandonment and emptiness. Social loneliness occurs, for example, when a person moves to a new place [33].

Knowledge about ways in which loneliness can be prevented and how feelings of loneliness can be reduced in older populations is urgently needed. Promising intervention strategies to reduce loneliness attempt to correct deficits in social skills, social support, opportunities for social interaction, and/or maladaptive social cognition [35,36,37,38,39]. Broader self-management abilities to maintain overall well-being could be considered to be important self-regulatory skills to handle these deficits and to combat loneliness in older populations. Steverink and colleagues [40] identified six self-management abilities: self-efficacy, positive frame of mind, investment behavior, taking initiatives, multifunctionality of resources, and variety of resources. Self-efficacy was originally defined by Bandura [41] and refers to the ability to gain and maintain a belief in one’s personal competence to achieve certain goals in life, such as reducing feelings of social and emotional loneliness. A person with stronger self-efficacy beliefs is more likely to undertake the activities and efforts needed to achieve such goals [40, 42]. According to Steverink and colleagues [40], having a positive frame of mind entails the ability to adopt and maintain a positive frame of mind or positive expectations, even after an illness or other major life event. The ability to stay positive despite certain challenging life events is expected to prevent feelings of loneliness because it extends the time horizon and boosts confidence, which encourages a person to engage in activities [40], and in turn may help prevent or reduce the worsening of feelings of loneliness. Investment behavior has proven to be crucial for achieving resource stability for older people [40, 42]. Without it, a (stronger) decline in resources is expected to increase the risks of social and emotional loneliness. Initiative taking refers to the ability to be instrumental or self-motivating, which is expected to prevent feelings of loneliness among older people; older people who often take initiatives are expected to report lower levels of loneliness compared with those who seldom take initiatives. The ability to gain and maintain a variety of resources entails having more than one resource or activity available to achieve a certain goal in life; for example, affection can be obtained from children, a partner, and a dear friend [40, 43]. Older people holding a variety of resources are expected to report higher levels of social and emotional well-being than are older people lacking such variety. Finally, multifunctional resources are resources and activities that serve multiple purposes simultaneously and in a mutually reinforcing way [40, 43], and are thus of special importance among older people who are experiencing a reduction of resources.

Although multiple studies have investigated the relationships between these six self-management abilities and well-being in older populations (e.g., [44,45,46]), we found few such studies that considered loneliness; one study investigated loneliness and the self-management abilities of self-efficacy and taking initiatives in visually impaired elderly people, and an intervention study developed by Steverink and colleagues incorporated all six self-management abilities and was conducted with older women [47, 48]. We currently lack research on the relationships between all six self-management abilities and social and emotional loneliness in a large representative sample of older persons. Given that loneliness is an urgent issue, especially in older populations, such research is crucial. This study thus aimed to investigate the relationships between the six self-management abilities and social and emotional loneliness among community-dwelling older people while controlling for background characteristics.

Methods

Study design and population

This cross-sectional study was conducted with a representative sample of community-dwelling older people in Limburg (a region in the southern Netherlands). The study population was selected from respondents to the Health Monitor survey, for which the Community Health Service sends an extensive general health questionnaire by mail every 4 years to a large national sample of community-dwelling people [49, 50]. In Limburg, the theme of self-management was added to this questionnaire, which could be filled in online or on paper. In 2016, questionnaires were sent to 41,327 people aged ≥55 years who were identified using the population register (weighted per district and using a complex sampling design). Selection from this sample was random for all represented age groups; people living in neighborhoods with low socioeconomic status were overrepresented. Two reminders were sent to non-responders, and the response rate was 50% (n = 20,327). On review, the medical ethics committee of the Amsterdam University Medical Center determined that the rules laid down in the Medical Research Involving Human Subjects Act did not apply to the Health Monitor study, and thus waived the need for further ethical approval. Via a contract between the Erasmus University Rotterdam and the Community Health Service South Limburg, Jane Murray Cramm and Anna Petra Nieboer obtained permission to use the data collected by the Community Health Service South Limburg.

Measures

Loneliness

We assessed social and emotional loneliness with the 11-item loneliness scale developed by De Jong Gierveld and Kamphuis [51]. Example items for the assessment of social loneliness are: “There are plenty of people I can rely on when I have problems” and “There are enough people I feel close to.” Examples of items used to assess emotional loneliness are: “I experience a general sense of emptiness” and “I often feel rejected.” Response categories are “yes,” “more or less,” and “no.” Item scores were dichotomized in agreement with the scaling procedure, with the response “more or less” indicating loneliness. Loneliness subscale scores were computed as the sums of dichotomized item responses, ranging from 0 (absence of emotional loneliness) to 6 (extreme emotional loneliness) for emotional loneliness and from 0 (not socially lonely) to 5 (extremely socially lonely) for social loneliness. The two scales have been proven to be reliable and valid for the assessment of social and emotional loneliness, respectively [21].

Self-management abilities

We measured older people’s self-management abilities with the Self-Management Ability Scale Short version (SMAS-S) [42]. This instrument measures the six self-management abilities of self-efficacy, positive frame of mind, investment behavior, taking initiatives, multifunctionality of resources, and variety of resources. Average SMAS-S scores range from 1 to 6, with higher scores indicating better self-management abilities. Cramm and colleagues [42] showed that the psychometric properties of the SMAS-S are good and that it is valid and reliable for the assessment of self-management abilities in older populations.

Background characteristics

We also asked respondents about their age, gender, marital status (single or not), and educational level [low (lower-level applied education), medium (medium-level applied education), or high (high-level education preparing for applied science or research university)]. We inquired whether they were chronically ill.

Data analysis

The data were analyzed using IBM SPSS Statistics 25.0 software (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA). The data were weighted to generate appropriate population estimates using a complex sampling design. Weighted percentages, means, and standard errors of the study variables were used in the reporting of study results. To identify relationships of background characteristics and self-management abilities to social and emotional loneliness, a general linear model with a complex samples design was employed for regression analyses (given the large sample, we used 0.01 instead of 0.05 as the significance level, default listwise deletion of missing cases was used).

Results

Table 1 displays descriptive statistics for the study population. Respondents’ mean age was 67.96 years, about half (47.9%) of them were male, and the majority (71.9%) were married. Mean social and emotional loneliness scores were 2.03 [standard error (SE) 0.2, range 0–5] and 1.51 (SE 0.02, range 0–6), respectively. Among self-management abilities, the lowest score was for the variety of resources (mean 3.66, SE 0.01) and the highest score was for investment behavior (mean 4.58, SE 0.01).

Table 1 Background characteristics and self-management ability and loneliness scale scores for the study population

In multivariate regression analyses corrected for background variables, all six self-management abilities were associated negatively with emotional loneliness, indicating that they protect against feelings of emotional loneliness among older people (Table 2). The strongest relationship was found between emotional loneliness and self-efficacy (β = − 0.64). Taking initiatives, multifunctionality, self-efficacy, and positive frame of mind were associated negatively with social loneliness, indicating that they protect against feelings of social loneliness among older people. The strongest relationship was found with self-efficacy (β = − 0.56).

Table 2 Relationships of background characteristics and self-management abilities to loneliness, as determined by regression analyses

Discussion

This research aimed to investigate relationships of broader self-management abilities to social and emotional loneliness among community-dwelling older people, while controlling for background characteristics. The results clearly show the importance of consideration of these abilities in efforts to understand loneliness in older populations.

All six self-management abilities were associated negatively with emotional loneliness, and four of these abilities were associated with social loneliness; self-efficacy had the strongest relationship with both forms of loneliness. These findings are in line with those of Alma and colleagues [52], who showed that self-efficacy and loneliness are related strongly in visually impaired elderly persons, and those of Fry and Debats [29], who showed that self-efficacy beliefs were a significantly stronger predictor of loneliness than were sociodemographic characteristics. These findings are important in a time of aging populations and high prevalence of social and emotional loneliness [8, 9]. Whereas earlier studies revealed relationships between loneliness and background characteristics (e.g., age, gender, marital status, educational level), this research adds to our knowledge by showing that self-management abilities remain associated with older people’s social and emotional loneliness after controlling for these background characteristics.

People’s social networks are known to contract, likely leading to a lack of quality relationships [32], as they grow older. Older people with stronger self-efficacy beliefs and positive and self-supportive thoughts, who actively invest in their social relationships, are better able to prevent feelings of loneliness than are older people with poorer self-management abilities. Older people who lack such skills communicate less with their neighbors and are less likely to seek help from others [53], which increases feelings of social and emotional loneliness, as well as the risk of social isolation. Investment in older people’s self-management abilities may help them to continue investing in their existing social relationships, and to find ways to create new relationships, which would help to prevent feelings of social loneliness. However, the avoidance of loneliness it is not simply a matter of the presence of others, but rather the presence of others who value one, whom one can trust, and with whom one can communicate [54, 55].

Older people with better self-management abilities are probably also better able to create the support system they need, including emotional, informational, and instrumental support, which prevents feelings of loneliness [56]. Cattan and colleagues [57] concluded in their review of intervention studies that the programs that reduced loneliness most effectively were group interventions. The organization of regular group meetings is a way to increase the number of one’s friends [58]. Moreover, educational and social activity interventions targeting specific groups can reduce social isolation in older adults [35, 57]. However, interventions should not focus on social relations alone [38]; more than one intervention strategy, including those that address maladaptive social cognition and enhance social support [38, 55], should be used. In accordance with these findings, Kremers and colleagues [47] reported improvement in self-management ability and social and emotional loneliness among single women aged ≥55 years participating in a self-management group intervention based on the self-management of well-being theory. The six self-management abilities defined by Steverink et al. [40] help older people to manage their resources in such a way that their overall well-being is maintained or even improved, and losses are avoided or coped with adequately. These behavioral and cognitive self-management abilities can be addressed in interventions [48] and improve social skills, social support, opportunities for social interaction, and/or maladaptive social cognition. Given their associations with social and emotional loneliness in the current study, this approach would be promising, especially among older persons with long-established and late-onset loneliness [9].

Although the heterogeneity of the tools used to measure loneliness prohibits direct comparison among studies, evidence suggests that the prevalence of “some degree of loneliness” [7], but not that of “persistent loneliness” (feeling lonely often, mostly, or always during the past week) [4], increased in recent decades. Indirect estimates of social and emotional loneliness, such as those based on De Jong-Gierveld and Kamphuis’ loneliness scale [51], do not increase over time, except for participants with activity limitations [16], and even decreased slightly among 64–84-year-old women in one study [6]. The variability of loneliness in later life calls for appropriately diverse interventions [9].

We distinguished between social and emotional loneliness on theoretical grounds [34], although empirically these concepts are highly correlated [38]. Dahlberg and McKee [59] showed that social and emotional loneliness are related but divergent constructs, with partially differing predictors. In the current study, taking initiatives, multifunctionality, self-efficacy, and a positive frame of mind were all important for both social and emotional loneliness, but investment behavior and the variety of resources were related only to emotional loneliness in multivariate analyses. Apparently, investment in an intimate relationship or close emotional attachment and the ability to gain and maintain a variety of resources to obtain affection protect against emotional, but not social, loneliness.

Several limitations of this study should be taken into account. First, the cross-sectional nature of the study prevents us from (1) showing how loneliness develops overtime (if it is stable or increasing) and (2) drawing causal conclusions. While our study showed that self-management abilities affect feelings of loneliness the relationship is dynamic; older people feeling lonely might view the world through “blue-colored glasses,” feeling less efficacious, have a less positive frame of mind, be less instrumental and/or less motivating. Both emotional and social loneliness are subjective appraisals which make the dynamic relationship plausible. Second, we found that self-management abilities are related to social and emotional loneliness among older people, but we did not investigate how these abilities can be improved nor did we look at age-related differences in activity patterns. Although promising interventions are currently available their longitudinal effects are still largely unknown. The large sample is a strength of this study, as is its representativeness of people aged 55 years or older in the province of Limburg, the Netherlands. Life expectancy in the Netherlands for men is (78.9) and women (83.2) [60].

Conclusions

In combatting loneliness among older people, investment in their ability to self-manage their social lives and activities, such as through regular socialization with family, friends and neighbors, seems to be crucial. Interventions that aim to enhance the self-management abilities of self-efficacy, positive frame of mind, investment behavior, taking initiatives, multifunctionality of resources, and variety of resources are expected to be useful additions to current public health interventions.

Availability of data and materials

Via a contract between the Erasmus University Rotterdam and the Community Health Service South Limburg, Jane Murray Cramm and Anna Petra Nieboer obtained permission to use the data collected by the Community Health Service South Limburg.

The datasets analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

SMAS-S:

Self-Management Ability Scale Short version

References

  1. Van Duin C, Garssen J. Bevolkingsprognose 2010–2060: sterkere vergrijzing, langere levensduur [population prognostication 2010–2060: ageing society, increasing life expectancy]. The Hague: Statistics Netherlands; 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Zantinge EM, van der Wilk EA, van Wieren S, Schoenmaker CG. Gezond ouder Worden in Nederland [healthy ageing in the Netherlands]. Rijks. instituut voor Volksgezondheid en Milieu: Bilthoven; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  3. European Commission. Third demography report. 2011. http://europa.eu/rapid/press-release_MEMO-11-209_en.htm.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Dykstra PA. Older adult loneliness: myths and realities. Eur J Ageing. 2009;6:91–100.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. OʼSúilleabháin PS, Gallagher S, Steptoe A. Loneliness, living alone, and all-cause mortality: the role of emotional and social loneliness in the elderly during 19 years of follow-up. Psychosom Med. 2019;81:521–6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Timmermans EJ, Hoogendijk EO. Broese van Groenou MI, et al. trends across 20 years in multiple indicators of functioning among older adults in the Netherlands. Eur J Pub Health. 2019;29(6):1096–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Arsenijevic J, Groot W. Does household help prevent loneliness among the elderly? An evaluation of a policy reform in the Netherlands. BMC Public Health. 2018;18:1104.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Grenade L, Boldy D. Social isolation and loneliness among older people: issues and future challenges in community and residential settings. Aust Health Rev. 2008;32:468–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Victor CR, Scambler SJ, Bowling A, Bond J. The prevalence of, and risk factors for, loneliness in later life: a survey of older people in Great Britain. Ageing Soc. 2005;25:357–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Cornwell EY, Waite LJ. Social disconnectedness, perceived isolation, and health among older adults. J Health Social Behav. 2009;50:31–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Hastings SN, George LK, Fillenbaum GG, et al. Does lack of social support lead to more ED visits for older adults? Am J Emerg Med. 2008;26:454–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Landeiro F, Leal J, Gray AM. The impact of social isolation on delayed hospital discharges of older hip fracture patients and associated costs. Osteoporos Int. 2016;27:737–45.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Nicholson NR. A review of social isolation: an important but underassessed condition in older adults. J Prim Prev. 2012;33:137–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Rico-Uribe LA, Caballero FF, Martín-María N, Cabello M, Ayuso-Mateos JL, Miret M. Association of loneliness with all-cause mortality: a meta-analysis. PLoS One. 2018;13:e0190033.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Valtorta NL, Collingridge Moore D, Barron L, Stow D, Hanratty B. Older adults’ social relationships and health care utilization: a systematic review. Am J Public Health. 2018;108:e1–10.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Honigh-de Vlaming R, Haveman-Nies A, Bos-Oude Groeniger I, de Groot L, van 't Veer P. Determinants of trends in loneliness among Dutch older people over the period 2005-2010. J Aging Health. 2014;26:422–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. De Jong Gierveld J. A review of loneliness: concept and definitions, determinants and consequences. Rev Clin Gerontol. 1998;8:73–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Dykstra PA, Van Tilburg TG, de Jong Gierveld J. Changes in older adult loneliness: results from a seven-year longitudinal study. Res Aging. 2005;27:725–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Heylen L. The older, the lonelier? Risk factors for social loneliness in old age. Ageing Soc. 2010;30:1177–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Jylhä M. Old age and loneliness: cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses in the Tampere longitudinal study on aging. Can J Aging. 2004;23:157–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Pinquart M, Sörensen S. Influences on loneliness in older adults: a meta-analysis. Basic Appl Soc Psych. 2001;23:245–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Tijhuis MAR, De Jong GJ, Feskens EJM, Kromhout D. Changes in and factors related to loneliness in older men. The Zutphen Elderly Study. Age Ageing. 1999;28:491–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Victor CR, Scambler SJ, Shah S, et al. Has loneliness amongst older people increased? An investigation into variations between cohorts. Ageing Soc. 2002;22:585–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Nieboer AP, Lindenberg SM, Ormel J. Conjugal bereavement and well-being of elderly men and women: a preliminary study. Omega. 1999;38:113–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Savikko N, Routasalo P, Tilvis RS, Strandberg TE, Pitkälä KH. Predictors and subjective causes of loneliness in an aged population. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2005;41:223–33.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Cohen-Mansfield J, Hazan H, Lerman Y, Shalom V. Correlates and predictors of loneliness in older-adults: a review of quantitative results informed by qualitative insights. Int Psychogeriatr. 2016;28:557–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Golden J, Conroy RM, Bruce I, et al. Loneliness, social support networks, mood and wellbeing in community-dwelling elderly. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2009;24:694–700.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Paúl C, Ribeiro O. Predicting loneliness in old people living in the community. Rev Clin Gerontol. 2009;19:53–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Fry PS, Debats DL. Self-efficacy beliefs as predictors of loneliness and psychological distress in older adults. Int J Aging Hum Dev. 2002;55:233–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Stephens C, Alpass F, Towers A, Stevenson B. The effects of types of social networks, perceived social support, and loneliness on the health of older people. J Aging Health. 2011;23:887–911.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Perlman D, Peplau LA. Toward a social psychology of loneliness. In: Gilmour R, Duck S, editors. Personal relationships 3: personal relationships in disorder. London: Academic Press; 1981. p. 31–43.

    Google Scholar 

  32. De Jong Gierveld J. Developing and testing a model of loneliness. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1987;53:119–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. De Jong GJ, Van Tilburg T. A 6-item scale for overall, emotional, and social loneliness: confirmatory tests on survey data. Res Aging. 2006;28:582–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Weiss RS. Loneliness: the experience of emotional and social isolation. Cambridge: The MIT Press; 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Alaviani M, Khosravan S, Alami A, Moshki M. The effect of a multi-strategy program on developing social behaviors based on Pender’s health promotion model to prevent loneliness of old women referred to Gonabad urban health centers. Int J Community Based Nurs Midwifery. 2015;3:132–40.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Bouwman TE, Aartsen MJ, van Tilburg TG, Stevens NL. Does stimulating various coping strategies alleviate loneliness? Results from an online friendship enrichment program. J Soc Pers Relat. 2017;34:793–811.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Chiang KJ, Chu H, Chang HJ, et al. The effects of reminiscence therapy on psychological well-being, depression, and loneliness among the institutionalized aged. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;25:380–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Masi CM, Chen HY, Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. A meta-analysis of interventions to reduce loneliness. Personal Soc Psychol Rev. 2011;15:219–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Stevens N, van Tilburg T. Stimulating friendship in later life: a strategy for reducing loneliness among older women. Educ Gerontol. 2000;26:15–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Steverink N, Lindenberg S, Slaets JP. How to understand and improve older people’s self-management of well-being. Eur J Ageing. 2005;2:235–44.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Bandura A. Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. Am Psychol. 1982;37(2):122–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Cramm JM, Strating MMH, de Vreede PL, Steverink N, Nieboer AP. Development and validation of a short version of the self-management ability scale (SMAS). Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2012;10:9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Nieboer A, Lindenberg S. Substitution, buffers and subjective well-being: a hierarchical approach. In: Gullone E, Cummins RA, editors. The universality of subjective well-being indicators, vol. 2002. Dordrecht: Kluwer; 2002. p. 175–89.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  44. Cramm JM, Nieboer AP. The importance of health behaviours and especially broader self-management abilities for older Turkish immigrants. Eur J Pub Health. 2018;28(6):1087–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Cramm JM, Twisk J, Nieboer AP. Self-management abilities and frailty are important for healthy aging among community-dwelling older people; a cross-sectional study. BMC Geriatr. 2014;14:28.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Cramm JM, Hartgerink JM, Bakker TJ, Steyerberg EW, Mackenbach JP, Nieboer AP. Understanding older patients’ self-management abilities: functional loss, self-management, and well-being. Qual Life Res. 2013;22:85–92.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Kremers IP, Steverink N, Albersnagel FA, Slaets JPJ. Improved self-management ability and well-being in older women after a short group intervention. Aging Mental Health. 2006;10:476–84.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Steverink N. In: Pachana NA, Laidlaw K, editors. Successful development and aging: theory and intervention, Oxford handbook of geropsychology: In; 2014. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199663170.001.0001/oxfordhb-9780199663170-e-028.

  49. Terstegge C, Houben T, Schefman S, Spee H, Hajema K, Mujakovic S, Quadvlieg M, Verberne N: Onderzoeksprotocol Limburgse monitor volwassenen en ouderen. GGD Limburg, 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Op het Veld LP, van Rossum E, Kempen GI, de Vet HC Hajema K, Beurskens AJ. Fried phenotype of frailty: cross-sectional comparison of three frailty stages on various health domains. BMC Geriatr. 2015;15:77.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  51. De Jong GJ, Kamphuis FH. The development of a Rasch-type loneliness-scale. Appl Psychol Meas. 1985;9:289–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Alma MA, Van der Mei SF, Feitsma WN, Groothoff JW, Van Tilburg TG, Suurmeijer TPBM. Loneliness and self-management abilities in the visually impaired elderly. J Aging Health. 2011;23:843–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Tadaka E, Kono A, Ito E, et al. Development of a community’s self-efficacy scale for preventing social isolation among community-dwelling older people (Mimamori scale). BMC Public Health. 2016;16:1198.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Cacioppo JT, Patrick W. Loneliness: human nature and the need for social connection. New York: W.W. Norton & Company; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Cacioppo S, Grippo AJ, London S, Goossens L, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness: clinical import and interventions. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015;10:238–49.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Rosen CE, Rosen S. Evaluating an intervention program for the elderly. Community Mental Health J. 1982;18:21–33.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  57. Cattan M, White M, Bond J, Learmouth A. Preventing social isolation and loneliness among older people: a systematic review of health promotion interventions. Ageing Soc. 2005;25:41–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Routasalo P, Tilvis R, Kautiainen H, Pitkala K. Effects of psychosocial group rehabilitation on social functioning, loneliness and well-being of lonely, older people: randomized controlled trial. J Adv Nurs. 2009;65:297–305.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Dahlberg L, McKee KJ. Correlates of social and emotional loneliness in older people: evidence from an English community study. Aging Mental Health. 2014;18:504–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Carolien H, Smits M, van den Beld HK, Aartsen MJ, Schroots JJF. Aging in The Netherlands: State of the Art and Science. Gerontologist. 2014;54(3):335–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank the respondents for filling in the questionnaires.

Funding

Not applicable.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

KJH contributed to the design of the study and data collection process. JMC performed the statistical data analyses. KJH, APN and JMC interpreted the findings. APN and JMC drafted the manuscript. APN, JMC and KJH all contributed to the refinement of the manuscript and approved the final version.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anna Petra Nieboer.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

We analysed an existing data set collected in a survey by others. The survey was not anonymous but confidentiality was assured during the data input phase. Thus, respondents’ personal data that could be used to identify the respondent were not available in the data set when it was provided to us for analysis. In the Netherlands, no ethics approval is required for such secondary data analysis. The medical research ethics committee of the Amsterdam University Medical Centre, the Netherlands, reviewed the research proposal of the original study ‘Gezondheidsmonitor Volwassenen en Ouderen 2016’ [Health Monitor adults and older people 2016] and determined that the rules laid out in the Medical Research Involving Human Subjects Act did not apply (d.d. 1 June 2016, number W16_166 #16.196). Via a contract between the Erasmus University Rotterdam and the Community Health Service South Limburg, Jane Murray Cramm and Anna Petra Nieboer obtained permission to use the data collected by the Community Health Service South Limburg.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

Jane Murray Cramm is an associate editor of BMC Geriatrics. Otherwise the authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Nieboer, A.P., Hajema, K. & Cramm, J.M. Relationships of self-management abilities to loneliness among older people: a cross-sectional study. BMC Geriatr 20, 184 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-020-01584-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-020-01584-x

Keywords