Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Cardiovascular risk factors and memory decline in middle-aged and older adults: the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing

Abstract

Background

We investigated the association between trajectories of verbal episodic memory and burden of cardiovascular risk factors in middle-aged and older community-dwellers.

Methods

We analysed data from 4372 participants aged 50–64 and 3005 persons aged 65–79 years old from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing who were repeatedly evaluated every 2 years and had six interviews of a 10-year follow-up. We measured the following baseline risk factors: diabetes, hypertension, smoking, physical inactivity and obesity to derive a cardiovascular risk factor score (CVRFs). Adjusted linear mixed effect regression models were estimated to determine the association between number of CVFRs and six repeated measurements of verbal memory scores, separately for middle-aged and older adults.

Results

CVRFs was not significantly associated with memory at baseline. CVFRs was significantly associated with memory decline in middle-aged (50-64y), but not in older (65-79y) participants. This association followed a dose-response pattern with increasing number of CVFRs being associated with greater cognitive decline. Comparisons between none versus some CVRFs yielded significant differences (p < 0.05).

Conclusions

Our findings confirm that the effect of cumulative CVRFs on subsequent cognitive deterioration is age-dependent. CVRFs are associated with cognitive decline in people aged 50–64 years, but not in those aged ≥65 years. Although modest, the memory decline associated with accumulation of cardiovascular risk factors in midlife may increase the risk of late-life dementia.

Peer Review reports

Background

Cardiovascular risk factors (CVRFs) (tobacco smoking, hypertension, obesity, physical inactivity and diabetes) are highly prevalent among midlife and older adults [1], and constitute leading causes of mortality. Of the 2.5 million Americans who died in 2005, tobacco smoking killed 1 in 5, elevated blood pressure was responsible for 1 in 6 deaths, and obesity and physical inactivity took 1 in 10 lives [2]. Besides this augmented risk for death, population-based studies have also identified CVRFs as strongly related to higher risk for accelerated cognitive decline [3, 4] and dementia [5]. For example, current smokers are between 50 and 80% more likely to develop Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) in the future, compared with those who never smoked [6, 7], and older adults who engage in high leisure-time physical activity have approximately 45% less risk of AD than people with the lowest level [8]. The effect of CVRFs on cognitive deterioration is mostly, but not exclusively, driven by cardiovascular diseases (CVD) [9]. Other pathophysiological mechanisms through which CVRFs might impact on cognitive decline or risk for dementia include inflammation and oxidative stress, cerebral small vessel diseases, cerebral hypoxia and hypoperfusion, or neurodegeneration in the brain, which in turn might also increase the risk for heart disease [10]. Several epidemiological studies have shown that the effect of several CVRFs on cognitive deterioration follows a dose-response pattern [10]. Thus, composite scores of CVRFs are often used as potential tools to detect at-risk older adults for cognitive decline and dementia.

Despite the association between CVRFs in older adults and risk for dementia being well-established [5, 9, 11], several authors postulate that this relationship might be age-dependent [12]. High blood pressure during midlife has been shown to be associated with an increased risk of AD in late life [13, 14], whereas research on the effect of high blood pressure in late life and dementia has yielded mixed results [9]. Similarly, the association of obesity and AD seems to be also age-dependent, with obesity in midlife reported as risk factor for AD [15], and underweight in late-life being more important in predicting AD [16]. Other studies have also demonstrated that aggregated CVRFs at midlife are associated with cognitive decline in the middle age [17, 18], suggesting that this effect can be observed at early stages. The associations between CVRFs during late life and dementia are less clear, with several studies finding very small or null effects on subsequent cognitive deterioration [13, 19].

Comparison across these studies focusing on middle-age versus late-life might be challenging because of distinct study designs, measures and sampling characteristics. To the authors’ knowledge, no studies have compared the impact of a summary score of CVRFs on subsequent cognitive deterioration in population-based middle aged versus older cohorts. The impact of a summary score of CVRFs at baseline on subsequent trajectories of cognition (verbal episodic memory) over a period of 10 years was investigated in a longitudinal, nationally-representative cohort of adults aged 50 to 64 and 65 to 79 years old. We aimed to study whether this association followed a dose-response pattern by exploring the interaction between time and CVRFs. Models were adjusted for several confounders to determine whether this association was above and beyond the effect of potential explanatory variables. The stratification of models by two age cohorts (middle-aged and older adults) allowed us explore age differences in the association between CVRFs and cognitive decline.

Methods

Study population

This study analysed data from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA), an ongoing project intended to collect data to study the dynamics of health, social, wellbeing and economic circumstances in the English population aged 50+, and more details about the study can be found elsewhere [20]. Briefly, it is a longitudinal, nationally representative survey of people aged 50 years and older living in private addresses in England. It was a randomized, stratified, multi-stage sample of participants [20] who earlier had taken part of the Health Survey England (1998, 1999 and 2001). The baseline sample (11,391 core members) was first assessed in 2002–03 and re-assessed every 2 years. Data are freely available from the UK Data Archive (https://beta.ukdataservice.ac.uk).

The present study used data from 6 successive waves of ELSA over 10 years of follow-up. We focused on core members who completed a non-proxy interview at baseline (n = 11,233) and excluded those who were aged 80+ at baseline (n = 10,026). Eighty-two participants who reported at baseline to have been diagnosed by a doctor with AD, Parkinson’s disease, dementia, organic brain syndrome, senility or any other serious memory impairment were excluded. Participants with information on verbal episodic memory score at baseline and at least in another wave and with complete information in all covariates (age, gender, educational and wealth level, marital status, number of CVD and non-CVD diseases) were included in the analysis, resulting in a final n of 7377.

Measures

Outcomes

The ELSA study includes a battery of cognitive tests to measure cognitive functioning in the elderly [21]. We focused on verbal episodic memory, which is related to every-day activities of older adults and has been shown to be sensitive to age-related decline [22]. The measure was assessed in the six waves using both immediate and delayed word recall tasks in which ten common words were presented aurally by computer to ensure standardised delivery. Participants were asked to recall them immediately and after a short delay, which was filled with other cognitive tests. There were four alternative lists, so that different lists could be given in distinct waves. The number of correct responses was recorded each time. This approach has been used elsewhere [21], and the word lists used here were those developed for the Health Retirement Study (HRS) [23]. The number of recalled words from both tests was added to obtain a total memory score (ranging from 0 to 20) with higher scores indicating better memory. The correlation coefficients between the immediate and delayed recall at baseline was 0.65. Immediate and delayed recalls have been known to have good construct validity and consistency [24].

Cardiovascular risk factor score

Following the literature [11], we generated a summary score, which included the following baseline CVRFs: self-reported hypertension and diabetes, history of smoking (current or ever vs. never), objectively measured obesity (Body Mass Index (BMI) > 30 kg/m2) and physical inactivity (not at all or mild physical activity vs. moderate or vigorous physical activity at least once a week). As BMI was not measured at baseline in 2002–03, we used BMI data that were measured in the Health Survey for England in 1998, 1999 and 2001. The CVRFs score initially ranged from 0 to 5. Due to the small number of people with 4 and 5 CVRFs, these categories were collapsed, and finally the CVRFs score had four categories, ranging from 0 to ≥3 CVRFs.

Covariates

Covariates were measured at baseline and included sex, age, marital status (never married, legally separated or divorced, married/remarried, and widowed), education (A-level or above recorded as high; O-level/Secondary education recorded as medium level; and no qualifications recorded as low education), quintiles of total net non-pension household wealth (including the net worth of savings and investments, property and business assets, but not pension-related assets), number of self-reported CVDs (angina, heart attack, congestive heart, abnormal heart rhythm, and stroke) and non-CVDs (chronic lung diseases, asthma, arthritis, osteoporosis, and cancer). Summary scores of CVDs and non-CVDs were generated. Depressive symptoms were assessed using the short 8-item Centre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) [25]. We calculated a summary CES-D score using all eight binary (Yes/No) symptom items. The CES-D summary score ranged from 0 to 8, with higher scores indicating more depressive symptoms.

Statistical analysis

Unadjusted and adjusted linear mixed effect regression models were estimated separately for the middle-aged (50–64 years) and older cohorts (65–79 years). The adjusted models included the covariates aforementioned. Time as continuous variable (from 0 to 10 years) was included in the models. To account for non-response, including survivor bias [26], we adjusted our models for a variable distinguishing between completers (participants who had complete memory data in all visits) and drop-outs (participants who missed at least one wave of ELSA for any reason including intermittent attrition, death and drop-out). To explore whether the rate of memory decline was different across the number of CVRFs, the interaction CVRFs*time was included in the adjusted models. Intercepts and slopes were measured as random effects. Since CVRFs are highly correlated with the presence of CVDs, adjustment for CVDs may attenuate the association of CVRFs with cognition. Thus, we also calculated the adjusted models excluding this variable (see Additional file 1: Table S1).

Marginal effects were used to graph linear adjusted predictions of verbal episodic memory over the follow-up while holding covariates constant. Post-hoc corrections (Bonferroni) for multiple comparisons were performed.

Analyses were conducted with SAS 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary NC) using the PROC MIXED procedure and Stata (SE version 13, College Station, TX). A two-side p value of less than 0.05 was considered as statistically significant.

Results

Table 1 presents a descriptive analysis of the total sample (N = 7377) and by age cohorts.

Table 1 Descriptive characteristics of the overall sample (N = 7377) and by age groups

Compared with the younger cohort, older participants presented higher rates of low education level (p < 0.001), and higher proportions of two or more non-CVDs and CVDs (p < 0.001). The prevalence of number of CVRFs was also higher in the older cohort (p < 0.0001). Mean score of episodic memory at baseline was lower than middle-aged people (p < 0.001). Number of depressive symptoms was also higher (p < 0.01). Some 22.5% of older participants died by the end of the study, and 45.6% completed all the assessments.

Trajectories of verbal memory score

Table 2 shows parameters from unadjusted and adjusted models in both samples. In the unadjusted models, there was a significant dose-response cross-sectional association between number of CVRFs and baseline memory function in both middle-aged and older adults, with higher number associated with lower memory scores (p < 0.0001). Longitudinally, there was a significant improvement in memory scores over time in middle-aged participants (p < 0.05), whereas a decline was observed in older participants (p < 0.0001).

Table 2 Parameter estimates, Standard Errors and p values from adjusted linear mixed regression models for episodic memory scores

After adjusting for covariates, in middle-aged participants, the cross-sectional association between CVRFs at baseline and memory score was no longer significant (p = 0.079). Participants who did not miss a study wave had better memory scores than those who missed at least one study wave (p < 0.001). Compared with the lowest quintile of wealth, higher levels of wealth were associated with better memory. Females, compared with males, and persons with higher levels of education had significantly better levels of memory (p < 0.001).

Regarding longitudinal changes in middle-aged persons, the interaction CVRFs*wave was significant (p = 0.004) indicating that different categories of CVRFs presented distinct patterns of decline over time.

For people aged 65–79 years, verbal episodic memory was still affected by time after adjustment, with a decline of 0.10 points every 2 years (p < 0.001), whereas the CVRFs were not associated with memory neither cross-sectionally not longitudinally; the interaction term between CVRFs and time were not statistically significant (overall significance p < 0.478).

The exclusion of CVDs from the adjusted models yielded similar results, as can be seen in Additional file 1: Table S1.

Figure 1 shows the trajectories of predicted means of verbal episodic memory over time for each number of CVRFs, stratified by age cohorts and adjusted for covariates. For middle-aged participants (Fig. 1a), those without CVRFs showed a significant improvement in memory scores over time, with a positive slope (b = 0.040, SE = 0.012, p < 0.01). By contrast, this improvement was not observed in those with one (b = 0.005, SE = 0.008, p = 0.55), two (b = − 0.005, SE = 0.01, p = 0.581) or three or more CVRFs (b = − 0.021, SE = 0.014, p = 0.125). Differences between the slopes of one, two and three or more, compared with none CVRFs, were all significant (p < 0.05 after Bonferroni correction). There were no significant differences when comparing slopes across one, two and three or more CVRFs.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Trajectories of predicted mean for verbal episodic memory in middle-aged and older participants Note: Predicted means were calculated from adjusted linear mixed models (see Table 2) while covariates were held constant. CVRFs = cardiovascular risk factors. a Middle aged participants, 50–64 years old (n = 4372). b Older participants, 65–79 years old (n = 3005)

Figure 1b shows adjusted trajectories of verbal episodic memory for older adults (i.e., aged 65 to 79). Again, slopes for each CVRF were estimated using marginal effects from the adjusted model in Table 2. Older adults with none CVRFs presented a significant decline over time (b = − 0.096, SE = 0.019, p < 0.001). A significant decline was also observed in those with one (b = − 0.131, SE = 0.012, p < 0.001), two (b = − 0.127, SE = 0.014, p < 0.001) and three or more CVRFs (b = − 0.123, SE = 0.016, p < 0.001). However, comparisons between these slopes yielded non-significant differences.

Discussion

Overall, our findings suggest a greater cardiovascular risk burden on contemporaneous memory decline in midlife, but not in late-life, supporting the hypothesis that the deleterious effect of cumulative CVRFs is age-dependent [10]. They also suggest that the effect of CVRFs follows a dose-response association with verbal episodic memory decline which is independent of other potential confounders.

As has been previously suggested, the presence of CVRFs in midlife is related to increased risk for late-onset cognitive impairment and dementia [27, 28]. Anstey et al. [17] speculated that, if these risk factors for late-life dementia occur in midlife, some effects on cognitive functioning, despite small, might be detectable in middle age. These authors found that greater CVRF burden, measured with a composite score of several risk factors, was associated with faster decline in reaction time. Similarly, other previous longitudinal population-based studies found worse cognitive performance associated with CVRFs in midlife [4, 18, 29]. In our study, middle-aged participants with no CVRFs show an improvement in their memory performance over 10 years. This lack of cognitive deterioration over time, or even some improvement due to practice effect, has been documented in previous research based on general populations, especially among younger and middle-aged adults [17, 30]. However, our results show that participants aged 50 to 64 years who had one, two or three or more CVRFs did not experience this improvement and did significantly worse compared with those free of CVRFs. Albeit subtle, the observed effect of CVRFs on verbal episodic memory is potentially important and meaningful, since one would expect memory not to deteriorate to an observable extent in middle-aged adults [31, 32].

Our findings also indicate that prospective association between cumulative CVRFs and memory in participants aged 50–64 years is dose-response with the memory score getting lower for every additional CVRF. The existing evidence concurs with that and suggests that composites of CVRFs have been shown to have a dose-dependent effect on the risk for dementia [33]. Since these risk factors frequently co-exist, intervention focusing on the combined effect of multiple CVRFs is preferable rather than focusing only on individual risk factors [3].

As for older adults (i.e., aged 65 to 79), we found that there were a significant decline of memory over time, but this was not associated with the number of baseline CVRFs. Previous literature has yielded mixed results when analysing the effect of summary risk scores of CVRFs in later life on risk of dementia, with some population-based studies reporting a positive effect [34, 35], and others failing to report significant effects of CVRFs in late-life on subsequent deterioration of cognitive function [5]. It has been also suggested that CVRFs no longer act as risk factors for dementia [10]. For example, obesity during midlife has been demonstrated to be a risk factor for late-life dementia, whereas for older ages, being underweighted is related to increased risk for cognitive impairment [12]. This U-shaped relationship has been similarly found for hypertension [36]. Higher levels of blood pressure at midlife which decrease over time has been shown to be associated with increased white matter lesions [37]. In a previous study conducted on the ELSA survey [38], we similarly found that the number of CVDs was only significantly related to subsequent latent trajectories of verbal episodic memory in midlife but not in late-life. A recent investigation with the ELSA study also reported that the co-occurrence of diabetes type II and elevated depressive symptoms was associated with accelerated cognitive decline, especially among those aged 50–64 years [39]. Therefore, it is possible that cognitive deterioration in older adults is being affected by different pathways or risk factors. Overall, middle-aged participants present with better levels of health at baseline than older participants. The number of non-CVDs and CVDs were significantly higher in the older cohort, and this might have greatly impact on their memory function. For example, musculoskeletal disease, lung disease or arthritis have been previously linked to cognitive decline [40]. Despite the fact that we controlled for the presence of multimorbidity (including both CVDs and non-CVDs), it is possible that the effect of chronic conditions on participants’ memory function might be more salient on the older cohort than in midlife, thus attenuating the detrimental effect of CVRFs in older participants. There is also the hypothesis that the duration of the exposure to CVRFs might impact on the rate of cognitive decline. Some previous research has supported this when studying diabetes in middle-aged adults [41, 42], hypertension [43], or smoking [44]. Future studies to investigate how age of onset of these CVRFs is impacting on cognitive functioning are warranted.

Survival bias might also partially explain this lack of differences. Presence of CVRFs in younger ages or midlife are related to higher risk for death [45], and thus older participants in our study might be healthier than expected. Cohort effects can also explain this lack of significance. Older and younger cohorts might be exposed to different risk factors or historical events during their lifespan [46].

Some limitations should be considered when interpreting our findings. First, some CVRFs at baseline were self-reported and they could have been affected by recall bias or low accuracy, especially for the older cohort [47]. Second, the age of onset for CVRFs was not considered in this study despite the fact that the duration of these conditions might play an important role on subsequent memory decline. Third, we focused on verbal episodic memory. CVRFs might affect differently the trajectories of other cognitive domains (e.g., attention, verbal fluency). Moreover, one could not discard floor effect, especially in the older age group. Fourth, despite the fact that we excluded those individuals who self-reported being diagnosed with dementia or other brain disorders, it is possible that some others might have presented with milder forms of neurodegenerative disorders, such as mild cognitive impairment (MCI). This would be particularly important if the presence of MCI is contributing to lower baseline cognitive scores and muting the test-retest effect at follow-up, especially in the older group. Finally, non-response might have also introduced some bias. However, we controlled for the confounding effect of completion.

Conclusions

Our findings support the deleterious effect of aggregate CVRFs in midlife (50–64 years) on subsequent decline over a period of 10 years, whereas this association was not found when CVRFs were measured in older adults (over 65 years). Despite being subtle, the decline observer among middle-aged adults with a high cardiovascular burden was significantly different from that observed for people with no CVRFs, where an improvement was observed. These differences in cognitive decline might increase as people aged, leading to greater risk for future dementia, such as AD. Interventions over these modifiable conditions at midlife could help stop this cognitive deterioration.

Availability of data and materials

The ELSA dataset is available in the UK Data service repository, https://discover.ukdataservice.ac.uk/catalogue/?sn=5050&type=Data%20catalogue

Abbreviations

AD:

Alzheimer’s Disease

BMI:

Body mass index

CES-D:

Centre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale

CVD:

Cardiovascular disease

CVRFs:

Cardiovascular risk factors

ELSA:

English Longitudinal Study of Ageing

MCI:

Mild Cognitive Impairment

SD:

Standard deviation

References

  1. Mozaffarian D, Benjamin EJ, Go AS, Arnett DK, Blaha MJ, Cushman M, et al. Heart disease and stroke statistics--2015 update: a report from the American Heart Association. Circulation. 2015;131(4):434 [Cited 2016 Aug 19]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25520374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Danaei G, Ding EL, Mozaffarian D, Taylor B, Rehm J, Murray CJL, et al. The Preventable Causes of Death in the United States: Comparative Risk Assessment of Dietary, Lifestyle, and Metabolic Risk Factors. Hales S, editor. PLoS Med. 2009;6:e1000058 [Cited 2017 Nov 17]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19399161.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Dregan A, Stewart R, Gulliford MC. Cardiovascular risk factors and cognitive decline in adults aged 50 and over: a population-based cohort study. Age Ageing. 2013;42:338–45. [Cited 2017 Nov 17]. Available from: https://academic.oup.com/ageing/article-lookup/. https://doi.org/10.1093/ageing/afs166.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Joosten H, van Eersel MEA, Gansevoort RT, Bilo HJG, Slaets JPJ, Izaks GJ. Cardiovascular Risk Profile and Cognitive Function in Young, Middle-Aged, and Elderly Subjects. Stroke. 2013;44:1543–9 [Cited 2017 Nov 17]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23640826.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Barnes DE, Yaffe K. The projected effect of risk factor reduction on Alzheimer’s disease prevalence. Lancet. Neurol. 2011;10:819–28 [Cited 2016 Aug 26]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21775213.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Anstey KJ, von Sanden C, Salim A, O’Kearney R. Smoking as a risk factor for dementia and cognitive decline: a meta-analysis of prospective studies. Am J Epidemiol. 2007;166:367–78. [Cited 2017 Nov 17]. Available from: https://academic.oup.com/aje/article-lookup/. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwm116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Peters R, Poulter R, Warner J, Beckett N, Burch L, Bulpitt C. Smoking, dementia and cognitive decline in the elderly, a systematic review. BMC Geriatr. 2008;8:36 [Cited 2017 Nov 17]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19105840.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Hamer M, Chida Y. Physical activity and risk of neurodegenerative disease: a systematic review of prospective evidence. Psychol Med. 2009;39:3 [Cited 2017 Nov 17]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18570697.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Qiu C. Epidemiological findings of vascular risk factors in Alzheimer’s disease: implications for therapeutic and preventive intervention. Expert Rev Neurother. 2011;11:1593–607 [Cited 2017 Nov 17]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22014138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Qiu C, Fratiglioni L. A major role for cardiovascular burden in age-related cognitive decline. Nat Rev Cardiol. 2015;12:267–77 [Cited 2016 Aug 19]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25583619.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Baumgart M, Snyder HM, Carrillo MC, Fazio S, Kim H, Johns H. Summary of the evidence on modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline and dementia: A population-based perspective. Alzheimers Dement. 2015;11:718–26 [Cited 2016 Aug 19]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26045020.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Qiu C. Preventing Alzheimer’s disease by targeting vascular risk factors: hope and gap. J Alzheimers Dis. 2012;32:721–31 [Cited 2017 Nov 17]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22842870.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Qiu C, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L. The age-dependent relation of blood pressure to cognitive function and dementia. Lancet Neurol. 2005;4:487–99 [Cited 2017 Nov 17]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16033691.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Alonso A, Mosley TH, Gottesman RF, Catellier D, Sharrett AR, Coresh J. Risk of dementia hospitalisation associated with cardiovascular risk factors in midlife and older age: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2009;80:1194–201 [Cited 2017 Nov 17]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19692426.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Gustafson D. A life course of adiposity and dementia. Eur. J. Pharmacol. [Internet]. 2008 [cited 2017 Nov 17];585:163–75. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18423446.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Atti AR, Palmer K, Volpato S, Winblad B, De Ronchi D, Fratiglioni L. Late-life body mass index and dementia incidence: nine-year follow-up data from the Kungsholmen Project. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2008;56:111–6. [Cited 2017 Nov 17. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01458.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Anstey KJ, Sargent-Cox K, Garde E, Cherbuin N, Butterworth P. Cognitive development over 8 years in midlife and its association with cardiovascular risk factors. Neuropsychology. 2014;28:653–65 [Cited 2016 May 5]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24635711.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Agrigoroaei S, Lachman ME. Cognitive functioning in midlife and old age: combined effects of psychosocial and behavioral factors. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc. Sci. 2011;66(Suppl 1):i130–40 [Cited 2016 Aug 19]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21743046.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Tolppanen A-M, Solomon A, Soininen H, Kivipelto M. Midlife vascular risk factors and Alzheimer’s disease: evidence from epidemiological studies. J Alzheimers Dis. 2012;32:531–40 [Cited 2016 Aug 26]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22842867.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Steptoe A, Breeze E, Banks J, Nazroo J. Cohort profile: the English longitudinal study of ageing. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42:1640–8 [Cited 2016 Jun 13]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23143611.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Huppert F, Gardener E, McWilliams B. Cognitive function. In: Banks E, Breeze C, Lessof C, Nazroo J, editors. Retirement, Heal relationships older Pers. Engl. English Longitud. Study Ageing (Wave 2). London: Institute for Fiscal Studies; 2006. p. 217–30.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Peters R. Ageing and the brain. Postgrad Med J. 2006;82:84–8 [Cited 2016 Mar 3]. Available from: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2596698&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Sonnega A, Faul JD, Ofstedal MB, Langa KM, Phillips JW, Weir DR. Cohort Profile: the Health and Retirement Study (HRS). Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43:576–85 [Cited 2018 Nov 21]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24671021.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Baars MAE, van Boxtel MPJ, Dijkstra JB, Visser PJ, van den Akker M, Verhey FRJ, et al. Predictive Value of Mild Cognitive Impairment for Dementia. Dement. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2009;27:173–81 [Cited 2018 Nov 21]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19202336.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Wallace RB, Regula A, Mary H, Ofstedal B, Steffick D, Fonda S, et al. HRS/AHEAD documentation report documentation of affective functioning measures in the Health and Retirement Study. 2000;

    Google Scholar 

  26. Weuve J, Proust-Lima C, Power MC, Gross AL, Hofer SM, Thiébaut R, et al. Guidelines for reporting methodological challenges and evaluating potential bias in dementia research. Alzheimers Dement. 2015;11:1098–109 NIH Public Access. [Cited 2017 Nov 20]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26397878.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Kivipelto M, Helkala EL, Laakso MP, Hänninen T, Hallikainen M, Alhainen K, et al. Midlife vascular risk factors and Alzheimer’s disease in later life: longitudinal, population based study. BMJ. 2001;322:1447–51 [Cited 2017 Nov 17]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11408299.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Stampfer MJ. Cardiovascular disease and Alzheimer’s disease: common links. J Intern Med. 2006;260:211–23 [Cited 2015 Aug 3]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16918818.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Knopman D, Boland LL, Mosley T, Howard G, Liao D, Szklo M, et al. Cardiovascular risk factors and cognitive decline in middle-aged adults. Neurology. 2001;56:42–8 [Cited 2017 Nov 17]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11148234.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Salthouse TA. When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiol Aging. 2009;30:507–14 [Cited 2016 Feb 17]. Available from: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2683339&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Craik FIM, Bialystok E. Cognition through the lifespan: mechanisms of change. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10:131–8 [Cited 2018 Nov 21]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16460992.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Tampubolon G. Cognitive Ageing in Great Britain in the New Century: Cohort Differences in Episodic Memory. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0144907 [Cited 2016 Feb 21]. Available from: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=4699214&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Beeri MS, Ravona-Springer R, Silverman JM, Haroutunian V. The effects of cardiovascular risk factors on cognitive compromise. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2009;11:201–12 [Cited 2016 Aug 19]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19585955.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Barnes DE, Covinsky KE, Whitmer RA, Kuller LH, Lopez OL, Yaffe K. Predicting risk of dementia in older adults: The late-life dementia risk index. Neurology. 2009;73:173–9 [Cited 2017 Nov 21]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19439724.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Reitz C, Tang M-X, Schupf N, Manly JJ, Mayeux R, Luchsinger JA. A Summary Risk Score for the Prediction of Alzheimer Disease in Elderly Persons. Arch Neurol. 2010;67:835–41 [Cited 2017 Nov 21]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20625090.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Ganguli M, Fu B, Snitz BE, Unverzagt FW, Loewenstein DA, Hughes TF, et al. Vascular Risk Factors and Cognitive Decline in a Population Sample. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2014;28:9–15 NIH Public Access; [Cited 2016 Aug 25]. Available from: http://content.wkhealth.com/linkback/openurl?sid=WKPTLP:landingpage&an=00002093-201401000-00002.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Vuorinen M, Solomon A, Rovio S, Nieminen L, Kåreholt I, Tuomilehto J, et al. Changes in vascular risk factors from midlife to late life and white matter lesions: a 20-year follow-up study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2011;31:119–25 [Cited 2016 Aug 26]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21273771.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Olaya B, Moneta MV, Caballero FF, Tyrovolas S, Bayes I, Ayuso-Mateos JL, et al. Latent class analysis of multimorbidity patterns and associated outcomes in Spanish older adults: A prospective cohort study. BMC Geriatr. 2017;17:186.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Demakakos P, Muniz-Terrera G, Nouwen A. Type 2 diabetes, depressive symptoms and trajectories of cognitive decline in a national sample of community-dwellers: A prospective cohort study. Ginsberg SD, editor. PLoS One. 2017;12:e0175827 [Cited 2017 Dec 4]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28414754.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Caracciolo B, Gatz M, Xu W, Marengoni A, Pedersen NL, Fratiglioni L. Relationship of subjective cognitive impairment and cognitive impairment no dementia to chronic disease and multimorbidity in a nation-wide twin study. J Alzheimers Dis. 2013;36:275–84 [Cited 2016 Apr 29]. Available from: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=4079077&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Tuligenga RH, Dugravot A, Tabák AG, Elbaz A, Brunner EJ, Kivimäki M, et al. Midlife type 2 diabetes and poor glycaemic control as risk factors for cognitive decline in early old age: a post-hoc analysis of the Whitehall II cohort study. Lancet Diabetes Endocrinol. 2014;2:228–35 [Cited 2018 Nov 21]. Available from: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S221385871370192X.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Xu W, Qiu C, Gatz M, Pedersen NL, Johansson B, Fratiglioni L. Mid- and Late-Life Diabetes in Relation to the Risk of Dementia: A Population-Based Twin Study. Diabetes. 2009;58:71–7 [Cited 2018 Nov 21]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18952836.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. TO O. Hypertension and Cognitive Function. Clin Geriatr Med. 2009;25:259–88 [Cited 2018 Nov 21]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19555872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Sabia S, Elbaz A, Dugravot A, Head J, Shipley M, Hagger-Johnson G, et al. Impact of smoking on cognitive decline in early old age: the Whitehall II cohort study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69:627–35 Inserm; [Cited 2018 Nov 21]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22309970.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Berry JD, Dyer A, Cai X, Garside DB, Ning H, Thomas A, et al. Lifetime risks of cardiovascular disease. N Engl J Med. 2012;366:321–9. [Cited 2017 Nov 20. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMoa1012848.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Keyes KM, Utz RL, Robinson W, Li G. What is a cohort effect? Comparison of three statistical methods for modeling cohort effects in obesity prevalence in the United States, 1971–2006. Soc Sci Med. 2010;70:1100–8 NIH Public Access; [Cited 2017 Nov 20]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20122771.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Huerta JM, José Tormo M, Egea-Caparrós JM, Ortolá-Devesa JB, Navarro C. Accuracy of Self-Reported Diabetes, Hypertension, and Hyperlipidemia in the Adult Spanish Population. DINO Study Findings. Rev Esp Cardiol. 2009;62:143–52 English Ed. Elsevier[Cited 2016 Aug 26]. Available from: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1885585709715324.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institute on Aging (Grants 2RO1AG7644-01A1 and 2RO1AG017644), a consortium of United Kingdom government departments (i.e., the Department for Education and Skills; Department for Environment, Food, and Rural Affairs; Department of Health; Department of Trade and Industry; Department for Work and Pensions; the HM Treasury Inland Revenue; the Office of the Deputy Prime Minister; and the Office for National Statistics, coordinated by the Office for National Statistics); and the European Union Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation Programme (ATHLOS-635316). BO’s work is supported by the PERIS program 2016–2020 “Ajuts per a la Incorporació de Científics i Tecnòlegs” (grant number SLT006/17/00066), with the support of the Health Department from the Generalitat de Catalunya.

The funding bodies did not have any role in the design of the study and collection, analysis, interpretation of data and in writing the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

BO contributed to the design of the study, statistical analyses, interpretation of data and wrote the main body of the manuscript; MVM conducted the statistical analyses and contributed to the interpretation of data; MB contributed to the design of the study, interpretation of data and provided feedback on the manuscript; JMH provided feedback on the manuscript; PD contributed to the design of the study, interpretation of data and wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Beatriz Olaya.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethical approval was obtained from the London Multicentre Research and Ethics Committee (MREC/01/2/91). Written informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Additional file

Additional file 1: Table S1.

Parameter estimates, Standard Errors and p values from adjusted linear mixed regression models for episodic memory scores (excluding cardiovascular diseases).

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Olaya, B., Moneta, M.V., Bobak, M. et al. Cardiovascular risk factors and memory decline in middle-aged and older adults: the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. BMC Geriatr 19, 337 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-019-1350-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-019-1350-5

Keywords