Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Cognitive training can reduce the rate of cognitive aging: a neuroimaging cohort study

Abstract

Background

The neural mechanisms underlying the restorative effects of cognitive training on aging brains remain unclear. To address this issue, we examined the relationship between changes in spontaneous brain activity and cognitive performance that occur after cognitive training.

Methods

Participants were older adults who were part of a randomized control trial within a larger longitudinal cognitive training study. We conducted single-domain and multi-domain cognitive training in two respective intervention groups. Participants were trained for 1 h, twice a week, for 12 weeks. Cognition was assessed in all participants and magnetic resonance images were obtained at baseline and 1 year after training. To assess spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity, we acquired resting-state fMRI data. Two indices—functional entropy and time-domain entropy—were used to measure the effects of training. Functional entropy increases with aging, and indicates disruptions in functional conectivity. Time-domain entropy decreases with aging, and indicates structural alterations in the brain and blood-flow reduction.

Results

Seventy participants completed the study: 26 in the multi-domain cognitive training group (70.38 ± 3.30 yrs), 27 in single-domain group (70.48 ± 3.93 yrs), and 17 in a control group (68.59 ± 3.24 yrs). Functional entropy increased significantly less in the multi-domain (p = 0.047) and single-domain groups (p = 9.51 × 10−4) compared with the control group. In the multi-domain group, this was true in the paracentral lobule (p = 0.004, Bonferroni corrected p < 0.05). Time-domain entropy also improved with training. Compared with controls, time-domain entropy in the multi-domain group decreased less in the inferior frontal gyrus pars opercularis (p = 3.59 × 10−4), the medial part of superior frontal gyrus (p = 1.17 × 10−5), and the thalamus (p = 4.72 × 10−5), while that in the single-domain group decreased less in the cuneus (p = 2.58 × 10−4, Bonferroni corrected p < 0.05). Additionally, changes in regional entropy for some regions such as hippocampus significantly correlated with improvements in cognitive performance.

Conclusions

Cognitive training can induce plastic changes in neural functional connectivity of healthy older people, and these changes may underlie the positive effect of cognitive training.

Trial registration

ChiCTR-TRC-08000732 (Date of registration: 5th November, 2008).

Peer Review reports

Background

Normal aging of the brain is associated with memory loss and cognitive impairments that can interfere with daily routines [1] and affect the capacity of older people to live independently. Additionally, cognitive impairment is associated with a higher mortality risk [2]. Therefore, researching how to prevent cognitive impairment with aging is essential, especially given the current aging crisis that is facing the world [3]. In addition to pharmacological therapies, the past few decades have seen a rise in research aimed at non-pharmacological intervention, such as cognitive training (CogTr) and physical exercise [46]. Increasing mental activity in older people by directed CogTr interventions is a particularly promising approach for delaying age-related cognitive decline [7]. Results from randomized controlled trials suggest that cognitive training can even induce improvements in cognition [811].

However, the neural mechanisms underlying the restorative effects of CogTr on aging brains remains unclear and are subject to debate [12, 13]. The essential difficulty, as pointed out in Owen et al. [12], lies in the difficulty of finding any evidence that the effects of CogTr transfer to untrained tasks. One promising way to fully assess the impact of CogTr on aging individuals and to explore its mechanisms, is to studying cortical changes (neural plasticity) via neuroimaging techniques such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) [14, 15]. fMRI is a noninvasive imaging method that assesses activity in regions of the brain based on the blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) contrast, and cognitive neuroscience research has already revealed brain plasticity throughout life that means changes and reorganization occurring even in the aging brain [16]. Cognitive training-related plastic changes in the brain have been reported in several task-state fMRI studies. For instance, activity in a number of frontal and parietal regions involved in memory, as well as activity in the bilateral hippocampus, was found to be greater after memory training in both healthy older people and those with mild cognitive impairment [15]. Further, semantic encoding-strategy training increased brain activity in the medial superior frontal gyrus, right precentral gyrus, and left caudate of older adults during intentional encoding [17].

Recently, using resting-state fMRI to complement traditional task-state fMRI is becoming more common. With resting-state fMRI, brain regions co-activated while not engaged in any specific task can be identified using spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity. Evidence increasingly suggests that temporal fluctuations in resting-state fMRI, in particular low frequency components (<0.1Hz), arise primarily from spontaneous fluctuations in brain metabolism and intrinsic neuronal activity [18, 19]. Researchers agree that spontaneous neuronal activity plays a fundamental role in brain function, maintaining brain integrity and its capacity to deal effectively with future demands [20, 21].

Exploring the effects of cognitive training on spontaneous brain activity in older adults may provide fundamental proof for the neural mechanism underlying the effects of cognitive training. Therefore, we conducted a neuroimaging investigation as a sub-study of the longitudinal CogTr program [11]. In the current study, we used two entropy indices, time-domain entropy and functional entropy, to quantify spontaneous neuronal activity. Entropy is a fundamental concept originating in statistical physics and information theory, and can be used to measure the degree of uncertainty within a probability distribution [22], with higher entropy reflecting greater uncertainty [23].

Time-domain entropy was used to characterize the uncertainty of individual time series of spontaneous brain activity that may indirectly reflect the complexity of brain structure. Aging has been found to be associated with a reduction in the time-domain entropy of spontaneous brain activity [2426]. Additionally, this measure of entropy was positively correlated with cognitive performance in the older people [25, 26]. A decreasing time-domain entropy with age (see Additional file 1 Section 3 for more details) reflects the lower oscillating frequency in BOLD signals as we get older, which can result from the loss of cortical and subcortical connections, loss of white matter or demyelination [27, 28], synaptic degeneration [29, 30], neurochemical alteration [31], or reduced blood flow [32]. In addition to time-domain entropy, the asynchrony of the time series among brain regions can be characterized by functional entropy, which can be viewed as an alternative expression of functional connectivity. Functional entropy measures the relative orderliness of the time series patterns among brain regions, with lower functional entropy values denoting greater conditional regularity or synchronicity. Yao et al. found that in a population of healthy individuals, functional entropy naturally increased with age, as evidenced by correlations in spontaneous brain activity that became more widely distributed [33], indicating age-related functional conectivity disruption. The first aim of our study was to determine if these two entropy indices change as a result of cognitive training.

Reduced lateralization of brain activity is another important feature observed in aging populations. This is known as “hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults” (HAROLD) [34]. According to this model, aging brains recruit additional brain regions to face task demands. This may reflect age-related dedifferentiation processes or compensatory functional brain responses [35, 36]. The second aim of our study was to explore the effect of cognitive training on age-related HAROLD.

Our hypothesis can be divided into three parts: 1) Cognitive training leads to changes in the entropy of spontaneous neural activity in healthy older adults, including reductions in the extent to which time-domain entropy decreases and in how much functional entropy increases. 2) Cognitive training reduces HAROLD. 3) Brain changes induced by cognitive training have correlations with cognitive performance. Because the effects of different cognitive training methods remain unknown, we investigated the effects of multi-domain and single-domain cognitive training.

Methods

The study was approved by the Human Research Ethics Board of Tongji Hospital in Shanghai, China. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants before the experiment (LL (H)-09-04).

Participants

Participants were from a longitudinal CogTr study. The longitudinal CogTr study was a randomized control trial in healthy older adults, using three groups, including two intervention groups (single-domain CogTr and multi-domain CogTr training) and one wait-list control group [11]. Details can be seen in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow of participants through the trial. ‘Refused’ or ‘Declined’ due to lack of interest in continuing, opposition from other family member, MRI examination in hospital recently, physical discomfort. ‘Died’ includes cancer, stroke, and cardiac sudden death

Participants were healthy older adults with normal functional capacity, living independently in the community. They were recruited between March 2008 and April 2008 from three community centers around Tongji Hospital through a notice/broadcast dispatched by the local community service institute. Eligibility criteria included: age 65–75 years; education ≥ 1 year; no difficulties with hearing, vision, or communication; no severe physical disease or psychotic disorder; and no obvious cognitive decline (a score on the Chinese version of the Mini-Mental State Examination ≥ 19 [37]). Exclusion criteria included: obvious cognitive decline such as AD; history or clinical evidence of neurological disease or psychiatric disorder such as brain cancer, major depressive disorder, or schizophrenia. Because the training-related changes predicted in our study have no relation with handedness [38], we enrolled both right- and left-handed participants. One of the 26 participants in the multi-domain training group was left-handed, as was one of the 27 in the single-domain training group.

Study design

We used a controlled and assessor-blind design to test the effect of CogTr on spontaneous brain activity and cognitive capacity. Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) was performed for all participants at baseline and at 1 year after training ending.

Interventions

We conducted single-domain or multi-domain CogTr in the two respective intervention groups. Participants completed 1 h of training, twice a week, for 12 weeks. The training procedure was administered in a classroom at Tongji Hospital. The multi-domain training targeted memory, reasoning, problem-solving strategies, visuospatial map-reading skills, and making handcraft. The single-domain CogTr focused specifically on reasoning, including the “towers of Hanoi” puzzle, numerical reasoning, Raven’s progressive matrices, and verbal reasoning. During the first 15 min of every training session, the trainer gave a lecture about a common disease in aging people such as hypertension. Then, the participants were taught about a certain cognitive strategy or technique via a slide presentation for the next 30 min. The training tasks were administered mainly via pen/paper. The newly learned skill was reinforced by solving a few real-life problems (for more details, see our previous study [10]). The control group did not undergo training and was used as a match for the social contact associated with CogTr. All participants including the control group were given a lecture about healthy living every 2 months.

Measures

Cognitive testing

All participants were, initially, given a cognitive capacity assessment, and this was taken as the baseline for any subsequent changes. One year after intervention another cognitive capacity assessment was conducted. The measures included the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status [39] (RBANS, Form A), which has been shown to have good reliability and validity in a sample of Chinese community-living older people [40], the Color Word Stroop test (CWST) [41], the Visual Reasoning test [42] and the Trail Making test (TMT) [43].

MRI acquisition

Scanning was performed using a Siemens 3.0 Tesla Allegra scanner (Erlangen, Germany) at East China Normal University, Shanghai, China. Foam padding was used to minimize head motion for all subjects. Functional images were acquired using a single-shot, gradient-recalled echo planar imaging sequence (repetition time = 2000 ms, echo time = 25 ms and flip angle = 90°). Thirty-two transverse slices (field of view = 240 × 240 mm2, in-plane matrix = 64 × 64, slice thickness = 5 mm, voxel size = 3.75 × 3.75 × 5 mm3), aligned along the anterior commissure-posterior commissure line were acquired. For each subject, a total of 155 volumes were acquired, resulting in a total scan time of 310 s. Subjects were instructed simply to rest with their eyes closed, not to think of anything in particular, and not to fall asleep. Subsequently, high-resolution T1-weighted anatomical images were acquired in the sagittal orientation using a magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo sequence (repetition time = 1900 ms, echo time = 3.43 ms, flip angle = 9°, field of view = 256 × 256 mm2, matrix size = 256 × 256, slice thickness = 1 mm, voxel size = 0.9375 × 0.9375 × 1 mm3 and 160 slices) on each subject.

Data preprocessing

The first 10 volumes of the samples were discarded to allow for scanner stabilization and subject adaptation to the environment. Preprocessing of fMRI data was then conducted using Statistical Parametric Mapping 8 (SPM8) (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm), Resting-State fMRI Data Analysis Toolkit (REST V1.8) (http://restfmri.net/forum/), and Data Processing Assistant for Resting-State fMRI (DPARSF V2.2) (http://restfmri.net/forum/dparsf_v2_2). The remaining functional scans were first corrected for within-scan acquisition-time differences between slices, and then realigned to the middle volume to correct for inter-scan head motions. Subsequently, the functional scans were spatially normalized to a standard template [44] (Montreal Neurological Institute) and resampled to 3 × 3 × 3 mm3. Data was then smoothed, and after normalization and smoothing, BOLD signals of each voxel were first detrended to remove any linear trend, and then passed through a band-pass filter (0.01 ~ 0.08 Hz) to reduce low-frequency drift and high-frequency physiological noise. Finally, nuisance covariates including head motions, global mean signals, white matter signals, and cerebrospinal signals were regressed out from the BOLD signals. After data preprocessing, the time series were extracted in each region of interest (ROI) by averaging the signals of all voxels within that region and then linearly regressing out the influence of head motion and global signals. In our present study, the automated anatomical labeling atlas [45] (AAL) was used to divide the brain into 90 regions of interest (ROIs) (45 per hemisphere). The names of the ROIs and their corresponding abbreviations are listed in Tao’s work [46].

Method for calculating entropies

After data preprocessing, time-domain data was extracted for each ROI by averaging the signals of all voxels within the region. We then calculated the Approximate Entropy [47] of the time series for each brain region with the following key parameters: window length, m = 1 time point; regularity of the time series, r = 0.12 × standard deviations. We termed the Approximate Entropy of each individual brain region the regional time-domain entropy. We proceeded to determine the average of all the regional time-domain entropies, and called this the time-domain entropy, following the rationale that different regional time-domain entropies could be considered as different realizations of the time-domain entropy.

We next calculated Pearson correlation coefficients for each pair of brain regions. The total number of correlations was 4005 because this is the number of functional links available from the AAL template. With these data, we constructed a whole-brain functional network. The 4005 different correlation coefficients require an indicator to represent features of the whole-brain functional network. We considered two possible approaches.

1) In the first approach, we took the values of the correlation coefficients as a realization of a random variable, which lies in the range [−1, 1]. We then assumed the brain entropy was the Shannon entropy [48]. Thus, we sorted all 4005 correlation coefficients into 20 class intervals of equal width, and determined the frequency (π) of each class. These frequencies were used to calculate the Shannon entropy for the whole brain, namely the sum of log (π). This can be considered the entropy of a discrete distribution, and following past usage [33], we called this result the functional entropy.

2) The alternative approach was to calculate the entropy for every single brain region. This method considers the functional links between a selected region and the other 89 regions. Thus, we could extract 89 Pearson correlation coefficients for each region by comparing its BOLD time series with those of the other 89 regions. We also separated these into 20 equal-width class intervals, and calculated the Shannon entropy of each region. We called the entropy of each brain region the regional functional entropy.

Statistical inference

In this paper, we used several statistical methods. The first and most basic one was the Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient. This was used to describe the relationship between each pair of brain regions and allowed us to quantify the relationship between the two types of entropy and other relevant quantities. Additionally, we used the Approximate Entropy [49] to extract the entropy of the time series. We used a Student's t-distribution to calculate the p value of the Pearson correlation coefficient, and used linear regression to remove the effects of age, gender, and education. We used the t-tests to compare measurements between each intervention group and control group. Additionally, a Chi-squared test was employed to test the comparability of gender, and an F test was utilized to test the comparability of age and education between the different groups.

We also collected cognitive behavioral data using the RBANS, CWST, visual reasoning tests, and TMT scores, and used before and after (1 year later) differences in these measures to calculate changes in cognitive ability. We used linear regression to determine the relationship between behavioral score changes and age, gender, and education in the control participants. We then took the control scores as a baseline and calculated the theoretical score-change in the multi-domain and single-domain training groups based on their age, gender, and education. We then compared the difference between the predictions and the actual values, and considered any difference to be an effect of the CogTr. We applied the same method when comparing the entropy values.

It should be emphasized that our results are primarily based on entropy—an informational concept—at the time and functional connectivity levels. To have a direct comparison with more conventional approaches, we also performed functional connectivity analysis and provide details in Additional file 1 section 8.

Results

Demographic information

Eighty-two participants responded and were assessed for baseline MRI scanning eligibility. Among them, 70 were eventually included in the MRI subsample. They were divided into three groups based on type of cognitive training: multi-domain (n = 26, mean age 70.38 ± 3.30 yrs), single-domain (n = 27, mean age 70.48 ± 3.93 yrs), and control (no training, n = 17, mean age 68.59 ± 3.24 yrs). Sixty-five participants completed the baseline MRI scanning. No significant differences in demographics were found between the participants who responded to advertisements and included in the imaging subsample. Fifty-four participants completed the 1-year follow-up. The dropout rate during the study was 28.57 %. The flow of participants through the entire study is illustrated in Fig. 1. The intervention groups were comparable with the control group with regard to age, gender, and education at each assessment. No significant differences in age, education, or gender were found across the three groups (p > 0.05). Subject characteristics are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1 Characteristics of participants at different time points in the study

Cognitive capacity

RBANS test score significantly improved (t = 2.75, p = 0.007) in the multi-domain CogTr group. Specifically, it was especially improved in language (t = 2.13, p = 0.024), attention (t = 1.95, p = 0.034) and delayed memory (t = 2.74, p = 0.007). In contrast, the single-domain CogTr group showed no significant changes. Moreover, CWST and TMT scores did not show any significant improvement in either group. Lastly, the visual reasoning score improved significantly in the single-domain CogTr group (t = 2.32, p = 0.017), and trended toward improvement in the multi-domain CogTr group (t = 1.61, p = 0.063).

Spontaneous brain activity

We found a number of differences between groups that were associated with spontaneous brain activity. The mean increase in functional entropy was significantly reduced in both multi-domain CogTr (t = 1.77, p = 0.047) and single-domain CogTr (t = 3.67, p = 9.51 × 10−4) groups. Additionally, the mean decrease in time-domain entropy was found to be smaller in both multi-domain (t = −0.77, p = 0.224) and single-domain CogTr group (t = 0.81, p = 0.787) when compared with the prediction for people with no training, as shown in Fig. 2.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Entropy change in cognitive training groups. The entropy differences (black bars) between the predictions (blue bars) and actual values (red bars) indicated the CogTr effect on the entropy measures. Panels (a) & (b): The mean decrease in time-domain entropy was found to be smaller in both multi-domain CogTr group (t = −0.77, p = 0.224) and single-domain CogTr group (t = 0.81, p = 0.787) when compared with the prediction for people with no training. Panels (c) & (d): The mean increase in functional entropy was significantly reduced in both multi-domain CogTr (t = 1.77, p = 0.047) and single-domain CogTr (t = 3.67, p = 9.51 × 10−4) groups. Subjects are ranked according to the entropy values

All regional time-domain entropies significantly decreased with age, as shown in Section 5 of the Additional file 1. At the regional level, time-domain entropy decreased significantly less in several brain regions of those in the multi-domain group compared with controls. In particular, this was true in the inferior frontal gyrus pars opercularis (t = 4.12, p = 3.59 × 10−4, Bonferroni corrected p < 0.05), the medial part of superior frontal gyrus (t = 5.75, p = 1.17 × 10−5), and the thalamus (t = 5.07, p = 4.72 × 10−5). Similarly, time-domain entropy decreased significantly less the cuneus of those in the single-domain training group compared with controls (t = 4.27, p = 2.58 × 10−4, Bonferroni corrected p < 0.05). In the Multi-domain training group, time-domain entropy in the amygdala (right hemisphere) correlated significantly with that in the middle orbitofrontal cortex (right hemisphere) (p = 7.2 × 10−6, Bonferroni corrected p < 0.05). However, time-domain entropy did not correlate between any pairs of regions in the single-domain training group (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Functional connectivity change in training groups. Panels (a) & (c) In the Multi-domain training group, time-domain entropy in the amygdala (right hemisphere) correlated significantly with that in the middle orbitofrontal cortex (right hemisphere) (p = 7.2 × 10−6, Bonferroni corrected p < 0.05). Panels (b) & (d) Time-domain entropy did not correlate between any pairs of regions in the single-domain training group

As shown in Yao et al’s work [33], regional functional entropies in the paracentral lobule, hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, olfactory cortex, and middle frontal gyrus, increase the most with age. In our experiment, functional entropy in the paracentral lobule increased less in the multi-domain training group than in controls (t = 3.03, p = 0.004, Bonferroni corrected p < 0.05).

Differences in functional entropy between left and right hemispheres decreased with age, as shown in Section 6 of the Additional file 1. In particular, asymmetry in functional entropy decreased significantly less in the multi-domain CogTr group than in controls (t = 2.48, p = 0.012). In contrast, it did not decrease less in the single-domain training group (t = 1.13, p = 0.136) (Fig. 4a and b). Additionally, asymmetry in gray matter decreased significantly less in the multi-domain training group (t = 2.93, p = 0.005; Fig. 4c) and single-domain training group (t = 3.33, p = 0.002; Fig. 4d). Details are shown in Additional file 1, Section 4.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Asymmetry in functional entropy change in training groups. Panel (a): The functional entropy asymmetry differences (black bars) between predictions (blue bars) with actual values (red bars) indicated that asymmetry in functional entropy decreased significantly less in the multi-domain CogTr group than in controls (t = 2.48, p = 0.012). Panel (b): In contrast, it did not decrease less in the single-domain group (t = 1.13, p = 0.136). Panels (c) and (d): asymmetry in gray matter decreased significantly less in the multi-domain group (t = 2.93, p = 0.005) and single-domain group (t = 3.33, p = 0.002). Subjects are ranked according to the values

Time-domain entropy in the middle temporal gyrus significantly increased with age, functional entropy significantly decreased with age in the orbital part of the superior frontal gyrus, and significantly increased with age in the putamen (Additional file 1, Section 7). Additionally, as shown in Fig. 5, compared to controls, the amount of entropy asymmetry was significantly reduced in the middle temporal gyrus (t = 1.72, p = 0.050), the orbital part of the superior frontal gyrus (t = 1.86, p = 0.040), and the putamen (t = 1.82, p = 0.043) of the multi-domain CogTr group. However, there was no similar significant reduction in the single-domain CogTr group.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Entropy asymmetry change in multi-domain CogTr group. Panels (a), (b) and (c): Compared to controls, the amount of entropy asymmetry was significantly reduced in the middle temporal gyrus (t = 1.72, p = 0.050). a the orbital part of the superior frontal gyrus (t = 1.86, p = 0.040) (b) and the putamen (t = 1.82, p = 0.043). c of the multi-domain CogTr group. Panel (d): These regions found in (a), (b) and (c) are represented. Subjects are ranked according to the entropy values

The correlation between cognitive capacity and resting state fMRI

We found that the change of regional entropy in some regions correlated significantly with improvements in behavioral data. As shown in Fig. 6, regional time-domain entropy for the hippocampus was significantly and positively correlated with delayed memory in the single-domain group (r = 0.760, p = 2.56 × 10−4) and positively (but not significant) correlated with delayed memory in the multi-domain group. In the multi-domain group, regional functional entropy of the calcarine fissure and the surrounding cortex was significantly negatively correlated with the visuospatial test (r = −0.758, p = 2.72 × 10−4). Additionally, entropy trends in the two regions were significantly reduced in the training groups (p = 0.050).

Fig. 6
figure 6

The correlation between cognitive capacity and resting state fMRI. Panel (a) and (b): Regional time-domain entropy for the hippocampus was significantly and positively correlated with delayed memory in the single-domain group (r = 0.760, p = 2.56 × 10−4) and positively (but not significant) correlated with delayed memory in the multi-domain group. Panel (c): In the multi-domain group, regional functional entropy of the calcarine fissure and the surrounding cortex was significantly negatively correlated with the visuospatial test (r = −0.758, p = 2.72 × 10−4). Panel (d): The regions referred to in panels (a) and (b) are shown

Discussion

In the results, we analyzed changes in the entropy of time series and functional connectivity from resting-state fMRI data. The trend of the functional entropy to increase, as observed in aging individuals, was significantly reduced in both groups that underwent cognitive training. Moreover, reduced lateralization, assessed by different entropy indices, was significantly suppressed in the multi-domain cognitive training group. Further, differences in gray matter changes were reduced in both training groups. Finally, behavioral improvements in memory and attention were significantly correlated with brain activity changes after training. In conclusion, our results provide explicit evidence that cognitive training can induce plastic changes at the level of intrinsic activity patterns in aging individuals. This is mainly through modifications in functional connectivity and brain structure, which are thus likely to be part of the neural mechanisms underlying the effects of cognitive training.

To our knowledge, this study is the first exploration of the effects of CogTr on functional connectivity in the aging brain from different aspects, as measured by entropy. Combining the analysis cognitive behavioral data, we conclude that while both types of CogTr could improve cognitive capacity in aging individuals, multi-domain training has more advantages in the cognitive domains that it affects.

We should emphasize that our results are primarily based on the informational concept of entropy, at the time and functional connectivity levels. To have a direct comparison with more conventional approaches, we analyzed the changes in functional connectivity between 4005 links in the different groups. We found one link that survived Bonferroni correction in the multi-domain training group, implying that connectivity was significantly improved with multi-domain training. The link in question was from the amygdala (right hemisphere) to the middle orbitofrontal cortex (right hemisphere). Li et al. explored the effects of multimodal intervention over a 6-week period, comprising cognitive training, Tai Chi exercise, and group counseling in the elderly, and found that intervention selectively affected resting-state functional connectivity between the medial prefrontal cortex and medial temporal lobe, which has some common features with our result. Differences could be because while they assessed short-term effects, we assessed long-term ones [50]. No links appeared to be correlated in the single-domain training group. Thus, consistent with the results from the entropy comparisons, multi-domain training is more effective than single-domain training. Perhaps it is not surprising that our entropy approach has revealed more information than the direct comparison using functional connectivity alone.

The results also suggest that the target of CogTr should be the dysfunction of cortical circuits that have been assumed to play a role in cognitive aging [51, 52]. The logic is given below. Extensive experimental data indicates that the number of neurons decreases significantly with age, as does the number of excitatory receptors (especially NMDA) [53]. Furthermore, changes in functional entropy with age can be simulated by computation models that include decreasing excitatory receptor number as a parameter [33]. CogTr may act to slow down the rate at which entropy increases with age by preventing the loss of the density of receptors on dendritic spines. This hypothesis could be tested in a diffusion-tensor imaging study.

At the regional level, training in the multi-domain group slowed down the decrease of time-domain entropy in the opercula part of the inferior frontal gyrus, the medial part of the superior frontal gyrus, and the thalamus. Similarly, training in the single-domain group significantly reduced the decrease in time-domain entropy in the cuneus. These results suggest that regions involved in speech production [54], self-awareness [55], relaying sensory and motor signals [56], and visual processing [57] are sensitive to CogTr. This study showed that in the multi-domain CogTr group, the paracentral lobule was where increasing functional entropy was significantly reduced. This is the medial portion of the superior frontal gyrus, continuous with the precentral and postcentral gyrus on the lateral surface. Salvador et al. found that functional connectivity for the paracentral lobule clustered statistically with resting-state activity of other frontal and parietal regions associated with spatial attention and motor functioning [58]. Therefore, improvement of functional connectivity in the paracentral lobule may be the neural basis for the improvement of attention capacity in the multi-domain CogTr group.

We also explored the effect of CogTr on the lateralization of time-domain and functional entropy. Lateralization can be viewed as a marker of brain activity efficiency. Consistent with a study having a larger dataset and wider age distribution [33], we found that functional entropy differences between the left and right hemisphere decreased with age. This decrease was significantly reduced in the multi-domain CogTr group, making the aging brain seem “younger”. Single-domain training did not have a similar result, suggesting that multi-domain CogTr could hold more advantages than single-domain CogTr. Multi-domain training might be enhanced by increasing the efficiency of multi-cognition domain-related regions and interactions between them.

At the regional level, the rate at which entropy asymmetry decreased was significantly reduced in the middle temporal gyrus, orbital part of superior frontal gyrus, and putamen of those in the multi-domain group. In fMRI experiments, these regions have been related to self-awareness [55], movement regulation [59], and several types of learning [60]. The results of the present study indicate that multi-domain training could increase the efficiency of those regions. Furthermore, we investigated gray matter hemisphere asymmetry using T1 image data. Consistent with studies of larger datasets with wider age distributions, the gray matter size difference between the left and right hemisphere significantly decreased with age in all three groups. However, the degree to which the asymmetry decreased was significantly less in the CogTr groups, thereby providing anatomical proof for the change in functional connectivity asymmetry.

To determine whether the entropy changes were relevant to behavior, we examined the relationship between the two. Changes in regional time-domain entropy of the hippocampus correlated significantly and positively with the change in delayed memory observed in the single-domain group, indicating the role of the hippocampus in the delayed memory process. In contrast, changes of regional functional entropy in the calcarine fissure and the surrounding cortex were significantly and negatively correlated with the change in visuospatial behavior observed in the multi-domain group. These regions function in primary sensory abilities such as vision and audition [61].

Several aspects of the present study should be taken into account when interpreting our results. First, the sample size was modest and may be susceptible to type II errors, obscuring true-positive effects. Bigger sample sizes are needed to verify the effect of CogTr on the aging brain. Second, the addition of a control group of young subjects could help determine whether CogTr training-induced changes are specific to older participants or whether they could also benefit younger adults. It would also help more directly determine whether the brains in the intervention groups get “younger” than those of the aging controls. Finally, it should be again emphasized that most of our results arise from an analysis based on the notion of entropy. We found that in the multi-domain training group, there was one functional link that was significantly modified, but there was no such link in the single-domain training group. In this sense, multi-domain training was better than single-domain training, and was consistent with the results based on entropy. Therefore, the entropy analysis revealed reliable results. We note that our entropy approach revealed more information than a direct comparison using functional connectivity analysis alone, which implies that entropy had more statistical power. We also conducted a functional connectivity analysis and found results similar to the entropy experiments (Additional file 1, Section 8). This could be attributed to global aspects of entropy. This is the reason entropy was selected to measure resting-state functional MRI.

Conclusion

In conclusion, CogTr can induce plastic changes at the level of intrinsic activity patterns in older people, mainly by modifying functional connectivity. Thus, changes in functional connectivity are likely to be a key neural mechanism underlying the CogTr effect. Multi-domain CogTr demonstrated more advantages than single-domain CogTr, perhaps because multi-domain CogTr involves more regions related to cognition. Larger sample sizes are urgently needed to study CogTr-induced changes in brain activity, cognition capacity, and their relationship.

Abbreviations

CogTr:

Cognitive training

BOLD:

Blood oxygenation level-dependent signals

fMRI:

Functional magnetic resonance imaging

ROI:

Region of interest

References

  1. Hedden T, Gabrieli JD. Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5(2):87–96.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Van Gelder B, Tijhuis M, Kalmijn S, Giampaoli S, Kromhout D. Decline in cognitive functioning is associated with a higher mortality risk. Neuroepidemiology. 2007;28(2):93–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Voss MW, Prakash RS, Erickson KI, Basak C, Chaddock L, Kim JS, et al. Plasticity of brain networks in a randomized intervention trial of exercise training in older adults. Front Ag Neurosci. 2010;2:32.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Geda YE, Roberts RO, Knopman DS, Christianson TJ, Pankratz VS, Ivnik RJ, et al. Physical exercise, aging, and mild cognitive impairment: a population-based study. Arch Neurol. 2010;67(1):80–6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Lautenschlager NT, Cox KL, Flicker L, Foster JK, van Bockxmeer FM, Xiao J, et al. Effect of physical activity on cognitive function in older adults at risk for Alzheimer disease: a randomized trial. JAMA. 2008;300(9):1027–37.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Pieramico V, Esposito R, Cesinaro S, Frazzini V, Sensi SL. Effects of non-pharmacological or pharmacological interventions on cognition and brain plasticity of aging individuals. Front Syst Neurosci. 2014;8:153.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Kelly ME, Loughrey D, Lawlor BA, Robertson IH, Walsh C, Brennan S. The impact of cognitive training and mental stimulation on cognitive and everyday functioning of healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res Rev. 2014;15:28–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Mahncke HW, Connor BB, Appelman J, Ahsanuddin ON, Hardy JL, Wood RA, et al. Memory enhancement in healthy older adults using a brain plasticity-based training program: a randomized, controlled study. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103(33):12523–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Wolinsky FD, Unverzagt FW, Smith DM, Jones R, Stoddard A, Tennstedt SL. The ACTIVE cognitive training trial and health-related quality of life: protection that lasts for 5 years. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2006;61(12):1324–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Anguera J, Boccanfuso J, Rintoul J, Al-Hashimi O, Faraji F, Janowich J, et al. Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults. Nature. 2013;501(7465):97–101.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Cheng Y, Wu WY, Feng W, Wang JQ, Chen Y, Shen Y, et al. The effects of multi-domain versus single-domain cognitive training in non-demented older people: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Med. 2012;10:30.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Owen AM, Hampshire A, Grahn JA, Stenton R, Dajani S, Burns AS, et al. Putting brain training to the test. Nature. 2010;465(7299):775–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Katsnelson A. No gain from brain training. Nature. 2010;464:1111.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Willis SL, Schaie KW. Cognitive training and plasticity: theoretical perspective and methodological consequences. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2009;27(5):375–89.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Belleville S, Clement F, Mellah S, Gilbert B, Fontaine F, Gauthier S. Training-related brain plasticity in subjects at risk of developing Alzheimer's disease. Brain. 2011;134(Pt 6):1623–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Gutchess A. Plasticity of the aging brain: new directions in cognitive neuroscience. Science. 2014;346(6209):579–82.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Kirchhoff B, Anderson B, Barch D, Jacoby L. Cognitive and neural effects of semantic encoding strategy training in older adults. Cereb Cortex. 2012;22(4):788–99.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Fox MD, Raichle ME. Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8(9):700–11.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Yan L, Zhuo Y, Ye Y, Xie SX, An J, Aguirre GK, et al. Physiological origin of low-frequency drift in blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Magn Reson Med. 2009;61(4):819–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Pizoli CE, Shah MN, Snyder AZ, Shimony JS, Limbrick DD, Raichle ME, et al. Resting-state activity in development and maintenance of normal brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(28):11638–43.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Uddin LQ, Supekar K, Menon V. Typical and atypical development of functional human brain networks: insights from resting-state FMRI. Front Syst Neurosci. 2010;4:21.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Wang QA. Probability distribution and entropy as a measure of uncertainty. J Phys A Math Theor. 2008;41(6):065004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Zhao F, Shin J, Reich J. Information-driven dynamic sensor collaboration. Signal Process Mag IEEE. 2002;19(2):61–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Liu CY, Krishnan AP, Yan L, Smith RX, Kilroy E, Alger JR, et al. Complexity and synchronicity of resting state blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) functional MRI in normal aging and cognitive decline. J Magnetic Resonance Imaging. 2012. DOI: 10.1002/jmri.23961.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Yang AC, Huang CC, Yeh HL, Liu ME, Hong CJ, Tu PC, et al. Complexity of spontaneous BOLD activity in default mode network is correlated with cognitive function in normal male elderly: a multiscale entropy analysis. Neurobiology of Aging. 2012. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.05.004.

  26. Sokunbi MO, Staff RT, Waiter GD, Ahearn TS, Fox HC, Deary IJ, et al. Inter-individual differences in fMRI entropy measurements in old age. Biomed Eng IEEE Trans. 2011;58(11):3206–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Head D, Buckner RL, Shimony JS, Williams LE, Akbudak E, Conturo TE, et al. Differential vulnerability of anterior white matter in nondemented aging with minimal acceleration in dementia of the Alzheimer type: evidence from diffusion tensor imaging. Cereb Cortex. 2004;14(4):410–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Salat D, Tuch D, Greve D, Van Der Kouwe A, Hevelone N, Zaleta A, et al. Age-related alterations in white matter microstructure measured by diffusion tensor imaging. Neurobiol Aging. 2005;26(8):1215–27.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Schliebs R, Arendt T. The cholinergic system in aging and neuronal degeneration. Behav Brain Res. 2011;221(2):555–63.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Dickstein DL, Weaver CM, Luebke JI, and Hof PR. Dendritic spine changes associated with normal aging. Neuroscience. 2012. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.09.077.

  31. Luiten P, Nyakas C, Eisel U, and van der Zee E. Aging of the Brain. In: Pfaff DW, editor. Neuroscience in the 21st Century. New York: Springer; 2013. p. 2239–72.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  32. Chen JJ, Rosas HD, Salat DH. Age-associated reductions in cerebral blood flow are independent from regional atrophy. Neuroimage. 2011;55(2):468–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Yao Y, Lu W, Xu B, Li C, Lin C, Waxman D, et al. The increase of the functional entropy of the human brain with age. Sci Rep. 2013;3:2853.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Cabeza R. Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: The HAROLD model. Psychol Aging. 2002;17(1):85–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Dolcos F, Rice HJ, Cabeza R. Hemispheric asymmetry and aging: right hemisphere decline or asymmetry reduction. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2002;26(7):819–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Berlingeri M, Bottini G, Danelli L, Ferri F, Traficante D, Sacheli L, et al. With time on our side? Task-dependent compensatory processes in graceful aging. Exp Brain Res. 2010;205(3):307–24.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Li C, Wu W, Jin H, Zhang X, Xue H, He Y, et al. Successful aging in Shanghai, China: definition, distribution and related factors. Int Psychogeriatr. 2006;18(03):551–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Mackey AP, Singley ATM, Bunge SA. Intensive reasoning training alters patterns of brain connectivity at rest. J Neurosci. 2013;33(11):4796–803.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Randolph C, Tierney MC, Mohr E, Chase TN. The repeatable battery for the assessment of neuropsychological status (RBANS): preliminary clinical validity. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1998;20(3):310–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Cheng Y, Wu W, Wang J, Feng W, Wu X, Li C. Reliability and validity of the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status in community-dwelling elderly. Arch Med Sci. 2011;7(5):850–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Van Boxtel M, Ten Tusscher M, Metsemakers J, Willems B, Jolles J. Visual determinants of reduced performance on the Stroop color-word test in normal aging individuals. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2001;23(5):620–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Xiao S, Yao P, Li X, Zhang M. Neuropsychological testing profiles of patients with Alzheimer's Disease and mild cognitive impairment: a case-control study. Hong Kong J Psychiatry. 2002;12(4):2–5.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Ashendorf L, Jefferson AL, O’Connor MK, Chaisson C, Green RC, Stern RA. Trail Making Test errors in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and dementia. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2008;23(2):129–37.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Fonov V, Evans AC, Botteron K, Almli CR, McKinstry RC, Collins DL. Unbiased average age-appropriate atlases for pediatric studies. Neuroimage. 2011;54(1):313–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. 2002;15(1):273–89.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Tao H, Guo S, Ge T, Kendrick KM, Xue Z, Liu Z, et al. Depression uncouples brain hate circuit. Mol Psychiatry. 2013;18(1):101–11.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Pincus SM. Approximate entropy as a measure of system complexity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1991;88(6):2297–301.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Shannon CE. Prediction and entropy of printed English. Bell Syst Tech J. 1951;30(1):50–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Pincus SM. Approximate entropy as a measure of irregularity for psychiatric serial metrics. Bipolar Disord. 2006;8(5p1):430–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Li R, Zhu X, Yin S, Niu Y, Zheng Z, Huang X, et al. Multimodal intervention in older adults improves resting-state functional connectivity between the medial prefrontal cortex and medial temporal lobe. Front Aging Neurosci. 2014;6:39.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Andrews-Hanna JR, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Lustig C, Head D, Raichle ME, et al. Disruption of large-scale brain systems in advanced aging. Neuron. 2007;56(5):924–35.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. DeCarli C, Kawas C, Morrison JH, Reuter-Lorenz PA, Sperling RA, Wright CB. Session II: mechanisms of age-related cognitive change and targets for intervention: neural circuits, networks, and plasticity. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2012;67(7):747–53.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Morrison JH, Hof PR. Life and death of neurons in the aging brain. Science. 1997;278(5337):412–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Dronkers NF, Plaisant O, Iba-Zizen MT, Cabanis EA. Paul Broca's historic cases: high resolution MR imaging of the brains of Leborgne and Lelong. Brain. 2007;130(5):1432–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Goldberg II, Harel M, Malach R. When the brain loses its self: prefrontal inactivation during sensorimotor processing. Neuron. 2006;50(2):329–39.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Sherman SM. Thalamus. Scholarpedia. 2006;1(9):1583.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Zeki S, Watson J, Lueck C, Friston KJ, Kennard C, Frackowiak R. A direct demonstration of functional specialization in human visual cortex. J Neurosci. 1991;11(3):641–9.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Salvador R, Suckling J, Coleman MR, Pickard JD, Menon D, Bullmore E. Neurophysiological architecture of functional magnetic resonance images of human brain. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15(9):1332–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Marchand WR, Lee JN, Thatcher JW, Hsu EW, Rashkin E, Suchy Y, et al. Putamen coactivation during motor task execution. Neuroreport. 2008;19(9):957–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Ell SW, Marchant NL, Ivry RB. Focal putamen lesions impair learning in rule-based, but not information-integration categorization tasks. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(10):1737–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Belliveau J, Kennedy D, McKinstry R, Buchbinder B, Weisskoff R, Cohen M, et al. Functional mapping of the human visual cortex by magnetic resonance imaging. Science. 1991;254(5032):716–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank Zhiqiang Xue and Fang Fang contributing to the cognitive training validity test. We thank other members of the research group: Jie Qian, Xiangwei Wu, Yikang Zhu, Hongfang Huang, Lanlan Zhang, Lina Zheng, Zhujun Li, Meijuan Wang, Feiyan Yin and Changqing Yang for providing assistance with to data collection. This study was jointly supported by grant 30770769, 81371505 from the National Natural Science Foundation of China, Science and Technology Commission of Shanghai Municipality (13dz2260500, 134119a2501), National Centre for Mathematics, Interdisciplinary Sciences (NCMIS) of the Chinese Academy of Sciences, the Key Program of National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 91230201), the China Scholarship Council (CSC) and Research Leadership Development Plan of Shanghai Health Bureau (XBR2011005). Moreover, JF is a Royal Society Wolfson Research Merit Award holder.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Chunbo Li or Jianfeng Feng.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

TL and YY drafted the manuscript. YY and BX analyzed the data. YC carried out the cognitive training and participated in the training design. WF and YS contributed to the participants’ enrollment and cognitive assessment. QWL contributed to the data collection and performed the preliminary data preparation. CBL conceived of and designed the study, and reviewed the manuscript. WYW conceived of the study, and participated in its design and coordination. JFF helped to analyzed and interpreted the data. DW, JFF, JJW and XYC reviewed the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Ting Li, Ye Yao, Yan Cheng, Chunbo Li and Jianfeng Feng contributed equally to this work.

Additional file

Additional file 1:

Cognitive training can reduce the rate of cognitive aging: a neuroimaging cohort study. (DOCX 443 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Li, T., Yao, Y., Cheng, Y. et al. Cognitive training can reduce the rate of cognitive aging: a neuroimaging cohort study. BMC Geriatr 16, 12 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-016-0194-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-016-0194-5

Keywords