Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

What are the effects of animals on the health and wellbeing of residents in care homes? A systematic review of the qualitative and quantitative evidence

Abstract

Background

There is some evidence to suggest that animal-assisted interventions can have beneficial impact for residents in long-term care, but the focus of the evidence has largely been on behavioural and psychosocial measured outcomes. Animals, either as companion animals or in the form of pet/animal-assisted therapy, may provide benefits in the form of social contact, as well as opportunities for sensory experiences and meaningful engagement not picked up by outcome tools. This review aimed to create a state-of-knowledge synthesis, bringing together qualitative and quantitative findings, on the impact of animal-human interaction on care home residents and care home staff.

Methods

Fourteen databases were searched from inception to July 2020. Forward and backward citation chasing of included articles was conducted. Screening was undertaken independently by a team of reviewers. Thematic synthesis and meta-analysis were used to synthesise the qualitative and quantitative data.

Results

Thirty-four studies, published in 40 articles (20 qualitative and 20 quantitative) were included. Five themes relating to resident wellbeing were identified in the qualitative evidence synthesis. These were animals as ‘living beings’, reminiscence and storytelling, caring (as ‘doing’ and ‘feeling’), respite (from loneliness, institutionalisation, and illness), and sensory engagement. A sixth theme related to staff perceptions and wellbeing, and a seventh to animal health and wellbeing. Maintaining identity was identified as an overarching theme. The majority of randomised trials had small sample sizes and were rated as low quality, mostly showing no evidence of beneficial effect. There was, however, limited evidence of a positive effect of pet/animal interaction on outcomes of loneliness, anxiety and depression, supporting the themes of respite and sensory engagement.

Conclusions

The presence of animals can significantly impact the health and wellbeing of some care home residents. Residents had meaningful relationships with animals and derived pleasure and comfort from them. Interacting with animals offered residents a way to maintain a sense of self in the care homes, and with support, residents with dementia could also express their identities. Facilitating residents to interact with animals as part of person-centred care may also help residents to feel ‘at home’ in the care home.

Trial registration

PROSPERO registration no: CRD42017058201.

Peer Review reports

Background

The number of older adults needing long-term care worldwide is on the increase. In the UK, in 2011 291,000 people aged 65 and over were living in care homes in England and Wales [1] and by 2020 this has risen to > 360,000 [2]. In the USA, latest estimates place over 1.3 million older adults living in long-term care [3] and similar rises are being seen throughout Europe [4]. In addition, people living with dementia comprise a large proportion of the resident population of care homes [5, 6]. Moving from independent living to residential/nursing care is a key transition in an older person’s life and can significantly affect an individual’s quality of life [7, 8]. A recent systematic review by Gardiner et al. suggested that as many as 61% of older people living in long-term care may be moderately lonely and around 35% may be severely lonely [9]. The recent restrictions on family visits and group activities as a result of COVID-19 infection measures has further highlighted the issue of social isolation and loneliness for those living in long-term care [10, 11].

A home-like environment is important to residents [12], and the presence of animals or pets may contribute to a feeling of less institutionalised living [13]. Companion animals and pet/animal-assisted therapy may provide people with access to different forms of social contact, as well as providing opportunities for sensory experiences and meaningful engagement. The systematic use of animals or pets in the context of therapy for the purpose of improved health and wellbeing is often referred to as animal-assisted intervention (AAI). AAI can encompass animal-assisted activities (AAAs), focussing on the motivational, educational and/or recreational benefits of animals or animal-assisted therapies (AATs), which tend to be more structured, goal orientated and planned and often led by a trained therapist [14]. Although AAA and AAT have separate definitions, there is often overlap between them in practice and studies do not always differentiate between them [15]. There may also be opportunity for human animal interaction from less organised activities in this setting, such as live-in pets and domestic animals kept by the home itself.

There has been a number of systematic reviews over the past 10 years assessing the impact of AAI on the health and wellbeing of older people in general [16,17,18]. Some have focused specifically on people living with dementia or cognitive impairment [19, 20] or have limited the review to dog specific AAIs [21,22,23]. The majority of reviews have had mixed findings and focused on quantitative outcomes. None to date have brought together qualitative and quantitative evidence relating to AAI specific to older adults living in long-term care. This review aims to create a state-of-knowledge synthesis on the impact of animal-human interaction on care home residents and care home staff. In particular we sought to answer:

  1. 1

    what are the experiences, views and perceptions of residents, families/carers and care home staff of interacting with animals in older adult residential care settings? (to be answered by the qualitative evidence),

  2. 2

    what are the measured effects of animals on the health and wellbeing of older people living in residential care and of the staff that care for them? (to be answered by the quantitative evidence),

  3. 3

    are there different approaches or interventions (i.e. resident pets, pet visitation programmes, group or individual format, spontaneous or guided interactions, short- or long-term) that are particularly appropriate for different groups of residents (i.e. those living with dementia)? (to be answered by both qualitative and quantitative evidence),

  4. 4

    what is known about the effects of human-animal interaction on the therapy/participating animal in care homes? (to be answered by both qualitative and quantitative evidence).

Methods

Our review used best practice methods of evidence synthesis [24] and was developed in consultation with three relevant professionals (a care home owner, a care home manager, and a veterinarian) who formed our Expert Advisory Group (EAG). A full protocol outlining the methods of this systematic review which followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines [25] was registered with International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO) CRD42017058201.

Search strategy

The search strategy was developed by an information specialist (AB) and used a combination of relevant controlled vocabulary terms (e.g. MeSH) and free text terms. The original search was for robotic pets and visiting pets/animals which explains the search strategy for MEDLINE which is shown in Additional File 1: File S1 (and Table S1). The following databases were searched from inception to April 2017, update searches were run in July 2018 and again in July 2020: MEDLINE All from 1946, Embase from 1974, APA PsycINFO from 1806, SPP (via Ovid), CINAHL Complete, AgeLine from 1978 (EBSCOhost), CDSR, CENTRAL, DARE (Wiley Online, Cochrane Library), ASSIA (ProQuest), Web of Science Core Collection (SCI-Expanded, SSCI, A&HI, CPSI-S, CPSI-SSH, ESCI), SCOPUS and ProQuest Dissertations and Thesis Global with no date or language restrictions. Forward and backward citation chasing of each included article was performed.

Study selection and eligibility criteria

Eligible articles had to report either i) the views, experiences and perceptions of animal interactions of older people resident in care homes, their families and carers and care home staff, or ii) the effects of animal interactions on health and wellbeing (including depression, agitation, loneliness and stress and quality of life), social interaction, engagement, physical function, behavioural symptoms, medication use and adverse events. Care homes was used as an encompassing term for residential care home, nursing home, and long-term care facility.

For studies describing views, experiences and perception, eligibility was restricted to qualitative studies using recognised methods of qualitative data collection (such as interviews, focus groups and observations) and of analysis (such as thematic analysis, grounded theory, and Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis). For studies reporting effectiveness, eligibility was limited to randomised controlled trials (RCTs), randomised cross-over trials and cluster randomised trials. Eligibility criteria were applied to all unique titles and abstracts by two researchers (RA, NO, SP or RW) independently. The full text of articles initially considered as meeting the inclusion criteria were retrieved and the eligibility criteria applied in the same way. Discrepancies at both stages were discussed and resolved with another reviewer (JTC) where necessary.

Data were collected using standardised, bespoke data extraction forms, piloted for use in this review. Data were extracted by one of four reviewers (NO, RA, RW and SP) and fully checked by another. Data extracted related to the study design, setting, participant characteristics, the human-animal interaction (animals involved, type of interaction, format, duration etc.), reported experiences and perceptions relating to the interaction and outcome measures.

Quality appraisal and risk of bias

We used the Wallace criteria [26] and Cochrane Risk of Bias Tool [27] to critically appraise the qualitative and quantitative studies respectively. Qualitative studies were appraised by two reviewers (NO, RA). Quantitative risk of bias was performed by one reviewer (RA) and checked by a second (RW), with discrepancies discussed and resolved with a third (JTC).

Data synthesis

We used thematic synthesis [28], an approach that draws on thematic analysis used in primary studies, to synthesise the qualitative studies. It is a three-stage process comprising coding of text line-by-line, identification of ‘descriptive’ themes and the development of ‘analytical’ themes. One reviewer (NO) coded lines of verbatim text labelled ‘results’ or ‘findings’ within the included studies. The text included participant accounts and author interpretations. Texts were coded to represent meanings inherent in the original manuscripts rather than to fit any pre-determined model or framework. Example codes included ‘animals created a positive atmosphere in the home’ and ‘petting animals helped residents to relax and calm down’. Groups of related codes were combined and systematically organised into descriptive themes. The descriptive themes were then re-interpreted inductively to develop analytical themes that, together, addressed review questions (i), (iii) and (iv). The codes and themes were examined and discussed a number of times among two reviewers (NO, RA), to ascertain similarities, differences, and connections between them [28]. We found that our discussions were aided by using a thematic network approach to structure and depict the themes (and the relationships between them) as a web-like network [29]. We arranged the descriptive themes into networks, grouped around the analytical themes, and then grouped these around a ‘global’ or ‘macro’ theme. The global theme, at the core of the thematic network, was the theme that summarised and interpreted the other themes within the network. As an illustrative tool, the thematic network emphasised the ‘fluidity’ of the themes and the ‘interconnectivity’ throughout the network [29]. The synthesis was guided by the ENTREQ (‘Enhancing transparency in reporting the synthesis of qualitative research)’ statement [30] (See Additional File 1: Table S2).

For the quantitative data, random effect meta-analyses were performed where we had sufficient data assessing the same outcome [31]. Pooling was performed on the outcomes measured immediately following the intervention. As we used a random-effects model for the meta-analyses, the weightings for each study were determined not only by the size of each study included, but also by between-study heterogeneity. Unadjusted summary data were used to calculate standardised mean differences (SMDs). As all the outcomes were continuous, pooled effects are reported as standardised mean differences with 95% confidence intervals. Where there were differences in the number of individuals contributing to baseline and follow-up summary statistics, we used the average sample size. Where data could not be pooled, a narrative summary was undertaken.

To bring the syntheses together, we adopted an interweaving approach [32]. We used deductive methods to draw together the findings within the qualitative and quantitative syntheses separately. We then sought to explore where the quantitative effectiveness data could help verify or contrast suggestions put forward by the qualitative data, and where qualitative experience data could help to explain why an intervention may be effective or not. The two lead authors (NO, RA) were immersed in the entirety of the evidence base and started the process of the overarching synthesis as the findings of the individual syntheses were developing. This method helps to highlight the similarities (and differences) in findings from the qualitative and quantitative evidence. To visually represent the overarching synthesis, we used the qualitative evidence synthesis as a framework and mapped where the quantitative evidence supported or refuted the data, an approach we have used previously [33].

Results

From the initial searches and update searches, 391 articles were selected for full-text review, resulting in 35 included full text articles. With an additional 5 articles found through supplementary searching, 40 articles (reporting on 34 studies) met the inclusion criteria (see PRISMA diagram in Fig. 1 for reasons for exclusion). The 34 studies included 16 qualitative studies and 18 randomised trials.

Fig. 1
figure 1

PRISMA

General study characteristics

A summary of the main characteristics of all included articles is provided in Table 1.

Table 1 Study Characteristics

Qualitative studies:

In total twenty qualitative papers (from 16 studies) were included in this review. Studies were carried out in the USA (n = 6), the UK (n = 4), Canada (n = 2), and one in each from Norway [39], New Zealand [51, 52], Japan [40] and Sweden [47,48,49,50]. Studies reported on the experience of a wide range of human animal interactions including: residential home pets and animals kept on site (e.g. chickens) [35, 41], personal pets of the residents [36, 38, 44], and animal assisted interventions such as dog therapy or small animal visits (e.g. kittens, rabbits) (n = 10). One study considered staff members’ experiences of a wide range of animals living at, or visiting, care homes [37]. Eleven of the studies had a primary focus on the effects and experiences from resident perspectives, three focussed on the experiences and perceptions of care home staff [34, 37, 42], and three studies (across six papers) explored multiple perspectives (e.g., experiences and motivations of the dog handlers, nursing staff attending therapy sessions and/or the residents). Five of the studies focussed on the effects/experiences for residents living with dementia [40, 47, 49, 50, 53].

Quantitative studies:

Twenty papers, reporting on 18 randomised trials were included in this review. Interventions were described as pet encounter therapy, pet-facilitated therapy, pet-assisted living, animal assisted intervention, animal assisted therapy, animal assisted activity or simply dog visits/therapy. The majority of studies were from the USA (n = 11), and the remainder were conducted in Norway [64], Italy [59], South Africa [63], Australia [69], Spain [58, 70] and Denmark [67, 68]. Nine of the studies had a specific focus on residents living with dementia [58, 60, 61, 64, 66,67,68,69,70]. The sample size of the studies were generally small, ranging from six [61] to 144 residents [59], with eleven of the studies involving less than 50 residents.

Fifteen of the studies involved dogs as the intervention, one study involved cats [61], one study assessed in-room canaries [59], and one study reported on the effect of kitten and rabbit visits [62]. Intervention duration varied from a one-off visit [61, 62], to interventions of three to six weeks [57, 63, 66, 67, 71,72,73], longer interventions of 8–12 weeks [54, 55, 59, 60, 64, 65, 69, 70] and in one study, nine months [58]. The approach of the intervention was either one-to-one [54, 55, 57, 58, 62, 65,66,67,68, 70,71,72,73], including one study in which the animals (canaries) lived with the residents in their room [59], or was group–based [58, 60, 61, 63, 64, 69]. Touching of, and interaction with, the animal was reported as actively encouraged in seven of the studies [59, 64,65,66,67, 69, 70]. There was only one quantitative study that involved resident animals [59], with the remainder assessing the impact of visiting animals.

Study quality

Qualitative studies

All of the studies had a clear research question, used appropriate study designs and adequately described how the data were collected. The sample was drawn from the appropriate population in all of the studies but was considered adequate in just over half of them; ethical approval and gaining informed consent from the sample were noted in most studies. Just over half of the studies noted a theoretical framework which influenced the study design. In a small number of studies, it was difficult to appraise data collection and/or analysis due to inadequate reporting [34, 41, 42, 44,45,46, 53]. Most of the reported findings were substantiated by the data shown (See Additional File 1: Table S3).

Quantitative studies

Overall the quality of the randomised trials was poor, see Additional File 1: Table S4 for a summary of the risk of bias across the studies. Less than half of the trials were assessed as being at a low risk of bias for random sequence generation, and only three studies reported on methods that demonstrated adequate allocation concealment [64, 67, 70]. Almost all studies performed poorly in terms of the blinding of participants and personnel involved, with only one study clearly reporting methods that suggest a low risk of bias for outcome measurement blinding [70]. All but one trial [68] was assessed as low risk on selective reporting, and all but two trials [54, 62] were assessed as a low risk of bias on reporting of outcome data. There was a high proportion of items in ‘other bias’ rated as unclear due to the presence of sizable gaps in reported information for the domains of whether the appropriate analyses were used and whether the baseline data were equal across groups prior to the intervention starting.

Qualitative synthesis

Five analytical themes were identified relating to resident wellbeing: animals as ‘living beings’, reminiscence and storytelling, caring (as ‘doing’ and ‘feeling’), respite (from loneliness, institutionalisation, and illness), and sensory engagement. The theme ‘respite’ was informed by Swall et al.’s study [48] and was broadened to encompass respite from loneliness and institutionalisation. A sixth theme related to staff perceptions and wellbeing, and a seventh to animal health and wellbeing. All of these themes grouped around the global theme of identity (see Additional File 1: Figure S1 for the complete network of identified themes).

In this section we present the seven themes and Table 2 shows which studies contributed to each theme. Examples of quotations to support the themes are in Table S5 (Additional File 1: Table S5). The global theme of identity brings the themes together and offers an interpretation which is the focus of the discussion section.

Table 2 Summary of overarching themes

Theme 1. Human-animal interaction – animals as ‘living beings’

Several studies highlighted that residents appreciated the special qualities of either their pet or visiting animal, or animals more generally [38, 45, 46]. In their discussions, residents attributed animals with ‘almost human’ qualities such as an ability to ‘know’ and ‘understand’. Authors believed that the ‘human’ quality or ‘human likeness’ of animals helped to create a bond between the animal and the resident, and intensified residents’ level of engagement ([45], p.39). Residents described how their pets could empathise with them and provide emotional support; authors suggested that this sense of support may be due to the ability of a ‘living being’ to respond to individual residents ([43], p.7). The responsiveness of animals to residents meant that some residents preferred engaging with their pet or visiting animal above other activities and other people [38, 47]. Pets could act as substitutes for human relationships: for example, one resident spoke of how she could ‘build a relationship’ with her cat and she described her as ‘family’ and a ‘best friend’ ([38], pp.1973–4). In some cases, residents spoke of animals as being almost superior to humans, praising their care for their young, their kindness, love and loyalty, and how they could teach humans to care and be kind [45, 46]. Comparing the behaviour of animals and people allowed residents to demonstrate ‘human frailties’: “I love animals so much (especially cats) because they don’t talk back” ([46], p.118). However, not all residents seemed to appreciate the special qualities of animals or their pets, as illustrated by Mrs Thomas: “I mean, she’s just a cat” ([44], p.8).

Theme 2: Reminiscence and stories of past pets/family/occupation

How animals helped residents reminisce was highlighted by many studies [38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49]. Reminiscing through stories of past pets, interwoven with stories of childhood and adulthood, was a way of connecting to past selves and maintaining a sense of self and identity. Interacting with visiting animals facilitated the recollection of past events and experiences with their pets or other animals. Memories of previously owned pets could emerge after prompting but there was also ‘spontaneous reminiscing’, from those living with dementia [42, 43, 46, 47, 49]. Residents shared both happy and sad stories: they recalled how animals could be a source of ‘domestic comedy’ and recounted humorous episodes from their childhood [35, 43]. For some residents, particularly those from rural backgrounds, reminiscing about pets was a stimulus to share stories of their experiences of working with, and caring for, farm animals [35]. Residents shared their sadness and feelings of loss when recalling past pets [47] and these feelings could influence how they responded to involvement with visiting animals: in one case a resident declined, explaining that animals were associated with sad memories of his past pets [46]. Also, for those living with dementia, memories of the visiting dog and dogs from their past lives could “in the long run create confusion” ([48], p.2227): “[s]witching between joyful and difficult—sad memories seemed to create uncertainty and fear over what was real and true” ([47], p.87). Past pets could often be a ‘connecting thread’ to a person they had lost such as a deceased spouse [44,45,46]. One resident explained that he had lost both his wife and his third Airedale dog at around the same time and that his current dog was a way of connecting with both [38]. The losses associated with living in a care home could be distressing and, as described by one resident, pets could, to some degree, compensate for losses experienced: “It takes the place of the people that have died” ([44], p.8).

For some residents, being a pet owner was an ‘enduring aspect of self’ and they could not “…remember a time when they had been without a pet or imagine a situation in the future when their pet might be absent” ([38], p.1969). Bringing their pets with them to the care home was an important factor in their choice of care home [38]. Those who had been unable to bring their pets with them spoke of how difficult it was to leave their pet—who they considered as part of their family—and how their pets had been given to family members or friends. One resident said, “…my cat…Princess…My daughter’s got her now…I hated to get rid of her, I’ll tell ya. But, you can’t do nothing about it” ([41], p.32). Visiting animals could prompt residents to talk about these pets and tell stories of how they came to live in the care home. These stories could be a mixture of regret about having to give up a pet and gratitude for the person taking care of it. Savishinky (1985) observed that some residents described their pets as being ‘on loan’ and that they expected to be reunited with them: “…pets are thus a way for such individuals to voice their perception of nursing home life as a temporary situation” ([46], p.125). Sometimes these stories also conveyed a subliminal message of feeling abandoned by family, as with one resident who had praised her family for taking on her pet beagle but then observed that “they kept the dog and got rid of me” ([46], pp.125–6). The current health and condition of their pets could be an important topic of conversation for residents and also suggests that pets were a ‘stimulus for contact’ between the residents and those caring for their pets.

Theme 3: Caring

Resident pets – being responsible and meaningful activity

Residents demonstrated their care for resident and visiting animals in different ways and according to their interests and abilities. For those residents who were living with their personal pets, caring was regarded as a personal responsibility that often satisfied their need to ‘care for something’ [36, 38]. Caring for their pet could also be a way of maintaining the role of carer and keeping that “part of themselves that they valued alive” ([44], p.8). In some cases, residents needed staff help with the practical care of their pets, and accepted that sharing responsibility with others meant that the pet was less dependent on them [38]. Caring for a personal pet in a care home could lead to various worries and concerns for residents such as the pet running away (particularly on moving into the care home) and overfeeding of the pet without their knowledge [44]. Communal animals in care homes offered caring opportunities for those residents who were able and wanted to engage in a range of tasks such as feeding, grooming, cleaning, and collecting eggs [35, 37, 41]. Caring for animals in these ways could provide residents with meaningful activities and “…a sense of purpose and a routine that was different to that of tasks and personal care that so dominated their lives as care home residents”([35], p.62). However, some residents were clear about not wanting caring responsibilities for resident animals on a daily basis due to their physical disabilities [41]. There were also examples in one study which showed that caring could be detrimental for both resident animals, in particular overfeeding, and for residents who cared ‘too much’ and found it difficult to adjust to life without the pet [41].

Visiting animals – feeling involved, affectionate and concerned

Residents could show their care for visiting animals through how they interacted with them and how they expressed their feelings of care for the animals. Caring seemed to be closely linked to having regular contact with a ‘known’ animal and having a perceived relationship with it [40, 52]. In one nursing home where three dogs visited over two years, residents developed a one-to-one relationship with the dogs, and gained ‘confidence’ in their ‘familiarity’ with them. Although they were not involved in their daily care, they indicated that they felt they had ‘close relationships’ with them and perceived themselves as having ‘a special role in the dogs’ lives’ ([40], p.46). The ‘developing familiarity’ with a particular animal could be ‘bidirectional’ ([52], p.2649), with residents getting to know the animal and the animal getting to know the resident. However, not all residents felt able to develop a relationship with, and care for, a visiting animal [45, 52] for various reasons—because the animal was not theirs, the brevity of the visits and the possibility of different animals on each visit.

Interestingly, living with dementia did not necessarily preclude residents from showing care for visiting animals: in one study, residents received visits from a therapy dog team and provided care through touch and gestures such as slow patting and putting their arms around the dog, and in conversation with and about the dog [47,48,49]. Residents were observed being affectionate towards the dog, “treating it as a precious living creature” ([49], p.4), and being concerned about any health problems that the dog may have had. However, residents could unintentionally mishandle animals “due to their diminished cognitive capacities” ([34], p.1245) and in one case, a resident became too possessive with the dog and attempted to pick it up to keep it away from other residents [42].

Theme 4: Respite

Animals could offer respite to residents in a number of ways: respite from loneliness; respite from institutionalisation; respite from the symptoms of illness; and relief from pain and anxiety at the end of life.

Respite from loneliness

Animals provided comfort and companionship for some residents, and was particularly important for those who had brought their pets to the care homes and regarded them as the one aspect of their lives that remained constant [38, 44]. Their pets seemed to counteract their loneliness and helped ease the transition to living in a care home. Pets also helped their owners make friends with other residents and staff, and residents appreciated that others liked and enjoyed their pets, and the care that staff showed towards their pets. This, in turn, could help residents feel that they were well cared for themselves [38, 44]. A shared interest in animals enabled some residents to build relationships with staff, and animals and their antics could be a’talking point’ that staff and residents ‘enjoyed together’ [35, 37, 40]. Similarly, the presence of animals could lead to enhanced interactions between residents themselves and a greater interest in each other [39, 40, 43]. However, not all residents had opportunities to socialise with each other while experiencing the animal visits with some being confined to their beds and rooms [51, 52]. There was also the possibility that residents could display jealousy towards each other during animal visits [48].

Animals could also facilitate social encounters with people from ‘outside the care home’ such as volunteers [40, 45, 46] who could be volunteering with animal welfare organisations [52], or trained animal handlers [39, 47, 49]. For example, the HENPOWER project was set up in care homes by older community volunteers who assumed henkeeeping roles and interacted with residents, important for those who felt “isolated and cut off from the world” ([35], p.70). Dog handlers recognised the importance of having ‘small talks’ with residents using “the dog as a medium for contact and dialogue” ([39], p.108). For many of the residents, the social encounters with volunteers and animal handlers could become at least as important as the companionship provided by the animals [39, 46, 52], and in some homes, the regular presence of volunteers and pets helped create a ‘family atmosphere’ [46].

Respite from institutionalisation

The presence of animals was described by a number of authors as creating a positive atmosphere and bringing an interesting dimension to the daily life and routines of the care homes [35, 38,39,40]. Residents reported seeing the resident dog daily [41], and in the HENPOWER project, hens were regarded by many as a positive addition to the care home, transforming the gardens into interesting places with residents reporting that they spent more time in the gardens [35]. Savishinky ([46], p.127) suggests that animals in care homes can ‘momentarily’ recreate ‘past domesticity’ in which residents can participate: “…pets are symbolic and literal embodiments of the more complete domestic experience that residents once had.” Anticipation of visits could break the monotony of care home routines and particularly, where there was a regular visiting schedule, residents expected the pets and looked forward to the visits [38, 45, 51, 52]. In one study, looking forward to the dogs visiting was described as the “most pleasurable part of their life in the nursing home” ([40], p.44). Some residents wished that the animal visits were more frequent or lasted longer [38, 43, 52], and reflected that the benefit was restricted to the ‘moment of interaction’, described by one resident as a ‘fleeting’ pleasure ([52], p.2650). Having regular visits with the same animal could help residents to maintain a ‘sense of reality’ ([45], p.37), and dog handlers also recognised that regular visits with a stable group of participants was important in helping them get to know the residents [39].

Respite from symptoms of illness

A few studies illustrated that the presence of animals offered residents, particularly those with dementia, respite from the symptoms of their illness [34, 35, 39, 47,48,49, 53]. According to Swall et al. (2017), engaging with the dog (and its handler), could enable or empower some residents to ‘step out of the shadows’ of dementia and distance themselves from its symptoms, show something of their previous abilities and in that moment, act like a ‘healthy’ or ‘whole’ human being. Being with a dog could give residents a ‘temporary presence of mind’ ([47], p.87) or ‘episodes of lucidity’ [74], which could then uncover memories of childhood, nature and animals. The encounters with animals were often described as ‘moments’ (e.g. moments of joy and calm) which would not last; Swall et al. (2016) observed that the symptoms of the illness would return a few weeks after the visits stopped. Some staff could see the value of creating ‘good moments’ despite the likelihood that many residents would not remember the visits, which according to Casey et al. (2018: p.1245), meant that “each encounter [was] in essence a first encounter” and therefore, did not lead to any ‘sustained gains’.

Relief from pain and anxiety [at end of life]

One study found that dog therapy could provide relief from pain and anxiety for people with dementia at the end of life [48, 50]. The dog handlers observed that the presence of the dog with its warm body had a calming effect, reducing hyperventilation and anxiety, as well as pain. They believed that the physical warmth of the dog and its calming presence helped residents ‘open up’ and talk about their imminent death with the dog and sometimes, with the handler too. It could also shift the focus from their situation of nearing the end of life to the dog and its wellbeing: one woman on her last night was concerned about the dog’s comfort, requesting that the window was shut so that it would not be cold. The dogs could also have a physical effect on the person such as when a man approaching death opened his eyes and started to pat the dog. The connection between the dog and the person in these moments was, as noted by the handlers, often because “the person and the dog had a history of visits together” ([50], p.68). However, the presence of a dog did not alleviate anxiety for all residents and the handlers noted that anxiety could be so strong in some, that they often ‘closed themselves off’ ([50], p.69).

Theme 5: Sensory engagement

Physical contact with, and watching, animals brought pleasure, joy and calmness

Many studies emphasised how the presence of animals offered residents a sensory experience, primarily by tactile and visual stimulation [34, 35, 37, 39,40,41,42,43, 45,46,47, 49,50,51,52,53]. Watching animals or physical contact with an animal and its body through stroking, petting and cuddling could generate both verbal and non-verbal responses from residents, and often acted as a ‘source of diversion’ [42]. Staff, animal handlers and volunteers alike, commented on how physical contact with animals elicited positive responses from residents, and in some cases triggered memories and conversation. Through sensory engagement with animals many residents, including those with dementia, gained pleasure and joy, and also a sense of calm and comfort [49]. Residents spoke of how physical affection was key to their enjoyment and in one study, residents preferred a resident dog over fish and birds because they were able to physically interact with the dog [41]. Staff noted the importance of animals providing residents with non-judgemental and unconditional love [42]. The physical contact with animals seemed to induce calmness and provide relief from anxiety, important for those residents who may have been agitated [34, 48], and particularly, for those at the end of life [50].

Theme 6: Staff

Three studies reported staff interest and enjoyment in visiting animals and on the ‘positive feeling’ created within the care homes [34, 37, 39]. Staff understood that animals could make a difference to the residents—particularly if they had ‘long been a part of their lives’ ([37], p.317)—promoting interaction and engagement, and decreasing responsive behaviours for those living with dementia [34, 39, 44, 48]. Some staff believed that the animal visits made their jobs easier, helping them deliver person-centred care with a smaller number of residents while others were occupied with the animals [34]. Staff also appreciated the work of the dog handlers [39] and the person-centred care they offered residents, which arguably, contributed to a more caring culture within the care homes [37, 48].

There were staff that felt the presence of animals was disruptive to the routines of the care home and could distract them from caring for the residents. This mostly related to the perceived additional work that resident pets would generate such as cleaning and feeding, and staff reluctance to accept these tasks opened up the potential for animal neglect [37]. Staff could also lack confidence in their abilities and knowledge to care for animals as was the case with the hens in the HENPOWER project [35]. That animals could jeopardise care home hygiene was a significant issue for staff; other issues included the potential safety hazards of residents falling over animals and residents being allergic to animals. Importantly, staff recognised that communal animals could have a negative impact on those residents “at risk of being disturbed or distressed by their presence” ([37], pp.324–5).

Some staff regarded animals visits as a ‘waste of time’ and were overwhelmingly negative [34]; often the successful involvement of animals in care homes depended upon the “enthusiasm and responsibility of individual staff members” ([37], p .323–4). A supportive care home manager with a positive attitude and who was willing to manage risk appropriately was also very important [35, 39]. Staff identified a lack of clarity about policy and practice in relation to communal animals living in care homes, and a lack of guidance about how to arrange or conduct animal visits [37]. Clearly, care home managers should lead in developing a care home policy about animals, and in the planning and discussion with staff about job roles and responsibilities for introducing, monitoring and caring of animals [37, 42]. A clear procedure for assessing animal suitability for the home, and procedures and processes to deal with the problems that arise from introducing animals into care home settings such as infection control was also identified as crucial. Good organisation was also valued by the dog handlers when they visited care homes and it “…mattered…that they felt expected at the nursing home and that the nurses and residents were prepared for the visits” ([39], pp.108–9).

Theme 7: Animal wellbeing

We found little evidence in the studies about the effects of human-animal interaction on the wellbeing of the animals [34, 37, 42, 47]. The dog handlers in one study believed that the dogs appreciated the visits to the care homes and were committed to ‘the task’: “a wagging tail showed that they were eager ‘to go to work’” ([39], p.106). One volunteer described how the interactions with residents had helped her dog recover from depression and in her view, “…pet sessions [could] be as therapeutic for animals as for the people they visit” ([46], p.122). The degree to which some visiting animals benefitted from their interactions with care home residents may also have been influenced by their training and preparation. Two studies reported on how the dogs and dog handlers used in the care homes received training (18 months in Sweden) and were tested for suitability before being certified [39, 47]. In contrast, the visiting animals in another study were brought from an animal welfare organisation, primarily to socialise the animals in preparation for adoption, and there was no formal procedure for determining the suitability of the animal [51, 52]. This had the potential for causing fear and stress to an animal due to volunteers’ lack of knowledge about it and its behaviour [75]. Clearly, not all animals are temperamentally suited to the care home setting and not all are suited to the role of visiting animal [76, 77].

Quantitative synthesis

Outcomes were grouped into three categories: psychological, behavioural and wellbeing. A detailed summary of the quantitative outcome results is shown in Table 3. Meta-analyses were only possible for the psychological outcomes of depression and anxiety, the behavioural outcome of agitation, and the wellbeing outcome of quality of life (see Additional File 1: Figure S2). A reduction in anxiety was the only significant outcome from pooled analyses. A narrative synthesis of the findings for comparable outcomes not able to be pooled and reported by more than one study is provided below.

Table 3 Summary of key outcome data from the randomised studies

Psychological

Depression and anxiety

Ten RCTs assessed the effect of pet therapy on depression [59, 60, 62,63,64,65, 67, 69, 71, 72]. The majority of the studies involved therapy/activity sessions with dogs, and sessions ranged from as little as 5-10 min/week to 2 X 90 min /week and occurred over a range of 3 to 12 weeks. One study involved assessing the effect of canaries that lived in the residents room [59], and another study looked at one off therapy sessions with kittens or rabbits [62]. Seven of the interventions were based on one-to-one interaction, and three used a group-based approach. Eight different validated depression assessment tools were used (see Table 3). Seven of the studies provided data that could be pooled for meta-analysis, involving a total of 173 residents in the intervention and 187 in the control groups. The pooled standardised mean difference (SMD) of effect on depression was 0.34 (95%CI -0.73 to 0.04; p = 0.08; I2 = 67%) (see Additional File 1: Figure S2). The three studies not able to be included in the pooled analysis individually reported no effect on resident depression.

Three RCTs reported on the effect of pet therapy on resident anxiety [59, 62, 63]. All used a one-to-one approach, and involved looking after a caged canary for three months [59], one-off sessions with kittens or rabbits [62] or a dog visit once a week for 6 weeks [63]. Anxiety was assessed using three different tools. The pooled results across the three trials, involving 158 residents, indicated evidence of a small reduction in anxiety, SMD -0.36 (-0.68 to -0.04, p = 0.03; I2 = 5%), see Additional File 1: Figure S2.

Loneliness

Two RCTs, run over 6–8 weeks, assessed the effect of dog visits on resident loneliness [56, 73]. Both studies had usual care (i.e. no visits from a dog) as the control group. Banks et al. (2008) compared the effects of a real dog with a robotic dog, against the control group and Banks (1998) compared two doses of dog visits (30 min/ week or 3 × 30 min/week). In both studies, loneliness was assessed using the same validated tool (University of California Los Angeles Loneliness Scale), however, there was insufficient raw data (one study presenting their data graphically) to formally pool the data. Both studies reported significantly less loneliness in those residents who received dog visits compared to residents in the control arms. There was, however, no difference in loneliness scores between the two doses of dog visits, or between the dog or robotic dog visits.

Morale

Two RCTs reported on the effect of dog visits on morale, both assessed using the Philadelphia Geriatric Centre Morale Scale [65, 72]. The results could not be pooled due to insufficient raw data being presented. Neither study reported any beneficial effect of a weekly dog visit on resident morale.

Behavioural

Agitation

Agitation was assessed in two 12 week parallel RCTs [60, 64], and one crossover RCT (of 2 weeks in each treatment arm) [66]; all three of which investigated the effects of weekly dog visits for residents living with dementia. Agitation was assessed using the Cohen Mansfield Agitation Inventory [60, 66] and the Behavioural Activity Rating Scale [64]. Pooling the data from the parallel trials showed there no evidence that dog visits had an effect on agitation, SMD -0.42 (-1.13 to 0.29;p = 0.25; I2 = 62%), see Additional File 1: Figure S2. The cluster randomised trial also reported no effect of dog visits on resident agitation.

Apathy

Two trials evaluated the effect of pet therapy on resident apathy [60, 70]. Both were studies assessing the impact of weekly dog visits over 12 weeks for residents living with dementia. The data could not be pooled due to insufficient reported raw data, however, both studies reported finding no effect of dog therapy on resident apathy.

General behaviour

Three studies reported on the effects of pet therapy using composite measures of resident behaviour [66, 70, 72]. All three were investigating the effect of dog visits, and two of the studies had a focus on residents living with dementia [66, 70]. The tools used included a bespoke social behaviour checklist [66], the Nurse Observation Scale for Inpatient Evaluation-30 tool [72], and the Neuro-Psychiatric Inventory [70]. The data was not able to be pooled due to insufficient reported raw data. The findings were mixed. Pope et al.reported significant improvements in overall behaviour in residents after two weeks of dog visits compared to visits with human interaction alone [66]. Valenti-Soler et al. found no significant difference in behaviour after 12 weeks of twice weekly dog visits compared with usual care visits with a therapist alone [70]. Zulauf reported significant improvements in one of the two groups receiving dog visits over 6 weeks, compared to therapist only visits, but the second group receiving dog visits were no different to the therapist only group [72].

Interaction/ engagement

Eight studies [54, 60,61,62, 66, 68, 71, 72] reported outcomes reflecting the effect of pet therapy on resident interaction and engagement, including verbalisation, degree of eye contact, smiling, interaction/touching, and observations of engagement and activity participation. Due to the diversity of measures, and lack of reported pre and/or post raw data, it was not possible to pool the data. The findings were very mixed and there was no consistent evidence of effect. For example, for verbalisation, three studies reported increased aspects of speech during pet therapy compared to control sessions [60, 61, 68] whereas others found no evidence of difference [54, 62, 71]. Similarly, greater levels of eye contact were observed during pet therapy in one study [68], but others found no difference [60, 62].

Wellbeing

Quality of life

Five studies assessed the effect of pet therapy on resident quality of life [58, 59, 64, 69, 70]. Four studies focussed on residents living with dementia. All four involved weekly dog visits, three over a period of 10–12 weeks and for the fourth over a period 9 months [58]. All four used a group-based approach for the intervention. Quality of life was assessed using the QUALID tool [64, 70] and the QOL-AD tool [58, 69]. The fifth study investigated the impact of the resident having either a personal bird or plant to care for, compared to nothing extra, over a period of three months [59]. In this study, the perception of quality of life was assessed using the LEIPAD-II scale. Data from two of the dog studies were able to be pooled [64, 70]. The pooled results show no evidence of effect from the dog visits on quality of life SMD -0.06 (-0.42 to 0.30; p = 0.75; I2 = 0%), see Additional file 1: Figure S2. The study by Travers et al. of 56 residents found mixed findings on quality of life across its three homes: no effect in one, improved quality of life in another and a reduction in quality of life in the third [69]. In the longer study by Briones et al., both groups improved with time, but there was no difference between those who had received dog-assisted therapy to those who had not [58]. The study by Colombo et al., found that having a bird to look after, resulted in significant improvement across all subscales of the quality of life tool, when compared to looking after a plant or usual care [59].

Overarching synthesis

The conceptual model, shown in Fig. 2, presents the synthesised themes identified through the qualitative synthesis and indicates where the quantitative evidence is supportive. Sensory engagement describes the experience that pets and animals had on the senses; with the joy, pleasure and the comfort that the sight and touch of a living animal can bring, along with associated memories that they stir. The pooled evidence from the trial data lends some support to this showing pet therapy can help to reduce anxiety and depression. Two studies also found evidence that pet therapy reduced loneliness. This may be through the sensory engagement that living animals enable but also resonates with the broader concept of respite, in this case respite from loneliness. In addition, one study of residents without dementia reported significant improvement in resident quality of life for those having a canary living with them in their room over three months, compared to being given a plant. This offers support to the theme of ‘respite’; in this case from institutionalisation, as the purpose of the trial was to give the residents something to care for, rather than them ‘being cared for’. This also connects with the theme ‘Caring’ and having ‘something to care for’.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Conceptual model of experiences and meanings of human-animal interactions in care homes

However, as described within some of the qualitative studies, there were instances in which residents did not engage with pets and visiting animals, nor appear to benefit from their presence and this was apparent in some of the trials. The pooled evidence for quality of life showed no overall effect, which stands in contrast to many of the areas within the themes of sensory engagement, respite and caring. In addition, there was no reported benefit from pet therapy on agitation for residents living with dementia in several studies, refuting perhaps the qualitative evidence of animals having a calming effect. There was also no evidence of effect on resident apathy in the trial data, again perhaps at odds with the concept of animals fostering meaningful activity, a strong theme in the qualitative data.

Whilst there were some trials that reported increased engagement and interaction, through speech, eye contact and physical touch, which could potentially lead to or reflect connecting with others and/or maintaining self, there were an equal number of studies that reported no impact. This did not appear to be related to whether the resident was living with dementia or not, nor any specific aspect or length of intervention. Similarly, results assessing the effect of pet or animal visits on various aspects of resident behaviour, were mixed within studies and across studies. There were no RCTS reporting outcomes that could support or refute the experiences of the Staff Perspective theme in the concept model.

Discussion

This is the first mixed methods systematic review to examine the experiences and effects of pets and animals in older adult residential care settings. A strength of the review is the rich qualitative findings which identified five themes around experiences and meanings of resident-animal interaction in care homes. Engaging with animals had positive effects on the health and wellbeing of residents by providing opportunities for residents to interact with ‘living beings’, to reminisce and share stories, to care for a living creature, have respite, and engage their senses. The conceptual model (see Fig. 2), generated from the process of drawing the qualitative and quantitative evidence together, depicts the resident-animal interaction at the centre, with identity as the global theme that draws the other themes together, and offers an overarching interpretative framework for understanding the health and wellbeing benefits of animal interactions for care home residents. In this section the discussion focusses firstly, on the significance of the relationship between animals and residents; secondly, on how residents maintain identity by living with, and caring for, their pet, reminiscing, participating in meaningful activities with animals, and through the sensory and embodied experiences with the animals; and thirdly, on the importance of care homes adopting a person-centred care approach to resident-animal interactions.

At the core of the conceptual model was the underpinning theme of the animal as a ‘living being’, and the kinship or connectedness that this meant to individual residents. Within human-animal interaction research, there is an implicit assumption that it is ‘something unusual, specific, or even unique about animals’ that brings about change [77]. In our conceptual model, the human-like quality of the animal underpinned the experiences of how pets and animals connected with the residents. Residents’ descriptions of animals as ‘almost human’ may be explained as anthropomorphism which is the “…attribution of human mental states (thoughts, feelings, motivations and beliefs) to non-human animals” ([78], p.437). Although regarded by some as ‘undesirable’, ([79], p.141)Beetz argues that it is the basis for understanding how humans relate to and communicate with animals and build social relationships with them. Residents, and particularly those who were living with their pets in the care homes, spoke about their relationships with their pets in terms of kinship [80], and how they received social and emotional support from their pets. Residents could interpret animal gestures such as a ‘paw on a cheek’ as a sign of emotional support and describe it in similar terms to that of human support. Their evaluations of the animal’s behaviour – often regarded as superior to that of humans – influenced the degree to which residents were attached to their pet. Arguably, relationships with animals were valued because animals were similar to humans and also because they were not [80]. The emotional bonds with their pets were of even greater significance to residents when they felt alone and apart from family and friends, and could even substitute for lost relationships (e.g. with a spouse). Thus, the support and positive social relationships residents experienced with their pets contributed to their wellbeing.

The residents benefitting from this social and emotional support were most likely to be those with a long-term relationship with their own pets and who were living with them in the care home. However, having a relationship with visiting animals was important for some residents too and familiarity could be achieved when the same animals returned on each visit, on a regular schedule over a sustained period of time [40]. Residents indicated that having the animals recognise them enhanced the pleasure they experienced, so regularity and continuity in animal visits is important to ensure that wellbeing benefits are delivered. Interestingly, in these relationships between humans and animals, the animal can be seen as an active participant with its behaviour and responsiveness shaping the relationship with the human [80]. This challenges those studies where the animal has been treated as the “…uniform variable that either is present or absent, as if all [animals] were equivalent, regardless of species, breed, temperament or behaviour” ([78], p.444). According to Serpell et al. (2017), acknowledging the individual characteristics of animals participating in interventions should be an important part of accounting for heterogeneity in RCTs and increasing their methodological rigour [77].

The presence of animals helped residents maintain continuity of self, particularly important as moving to, and living in, a care home can mean that residents experience changes and losses that impacts their identity and wellbeing [81]. For example, residents may experience loss of self-care ability, loss of control or autonomy, loss of meaningful connections [44], and loss of domesticity [46]. In this context, being able to keep their pet in the care home seemed to “…take on increased significance and meaning” for residents ([4], p.9), perhaps by helping residents to maintain a connection between their former life and current one in the care home [44, 82]. However, with many care homes not able to take on animals, some residents were forced to part with their companion animal, which Fox and Ray suggest is akin to the loss of a family member, having a detrimental impact on their health and wellbeing [76]. Residents’ descriptions of their pets as family indicates ‘connectedness and belonging’ and arguably, pets offered emotional support, comfort and security to such an extent that they provided residents with a sense of ‘ontological security’ [80]. That some residents were able to continue caring for their pets and fulfil their caring responsibilities was important, not only for maintaining a degree of autonomy or agency [36] but also for maintaining the identity of ‘pet owner’. Arguably, personal pets are part of the identities of residents in a way that communal and visiting animals could never be and to some extent, resident disinterest in visiting animals may have been recognition that the animal was ‘not theirs’ to which they could link their identity.

However, there is evidence to suggest that visiting and communal animals did enable some residents to reconnect with their former lives through reminiscence and storytelling, and through caring activities. Interestingly, some residents spoke of how important the visiting animals were to them in terms of ‘belonging to something’ [52]. Where residents were involved in the everyday caring routines for communal animals, they were ‘doing’ meaningful activities which may have been a way of linking with former routines and replacing the loss of other responsibilities [81]. For some, such activities could be described as ‘identity work’ [83], and meaningful activities have been identified as supporting personhood for residents with dementia by enabling them “to continue to be who they are” ([84], p.12). In some cases, there was an assumption that for residents to find interacting with the animal meaningful, they had to have experience of animals in the past, but this was not always the case [39, 48], which fits with Strick et al.’s assertion that ‘meaningful occupation’ can also support an ‘evolving and changing identity’ [85].

The importance of touch in resident encounters with animals, and the pleasure and comfort derived from touch, particularly for those residents with dementia, highlights how it can enhance the wellbeing of care home residents. In care settings, residents can experience ‘touch deprivation’ [86], in that they are more likely to receive instrumental, task-oriented touch associated with the routine tasks of caring, than caring touch for ‘reassurance or comfort’ ([87], p.543). Touch in the context of carers providing daily assistance with washing, dressing, eating and walking has been described as ‘bodywork’ [88]. That residents enjoyed the animals’ ‘snuggles’ and ‘cuddles’ suggest that such physical interactions restored an ‘element of touch’ to their lives ([46], p.115), and may even have acted as a “substitute for the lack of affectionate human touch” ([89], p.10). For some residents with dementia, cuddling and petting the animals enabled them to connect to the present and be aware of the animal and its body. In that moment, residents could engage in conversation, show awareness of, and respond to, the situation and express care for the animal through an “embodied knowledge of how to take care” ([90], p.329). This resonates with the concept of embodied selfhood [91] and how people with dementia use non-verbal behaviour to engage with, and connect to, the world.

The physical contact with an animal could also enable residents to connect with ‘life past’ and evoke ‘dormant’ memories and feelings which could bring sadness as well as joy [47]. Swall et al. draws on embodiment to explain how physical contact with animals could stimulate memory:

“To feel, see and hear the dog reveals feelings and expressions from a ‘whole’ human being, allowing them to connect with…memories earlier in life when they were younger and before they contracted AD [Alzheimer’s Disease]. Their existence and awareness might be due to the sense of being a ‘whole’ human being with the dog felt through their senses, which in turn connects with inner feelings and memories that they reveal when the dog is close”. ([47], p.89).

Downs argues that embodiment is important for understanding ‘continuity of self’ in dementia and the embodied nature of the human-animal interaction enabled residents living with dementia to express something of their identity [92]. There is increasing recognition that the body and embodiment are central to living with dementia with implication for care practices which, according to Downs ([92], p.368), should be “…person-centred in the sense of affirming personhood and sense of self.” Providing opportunities for residents with dementia to interact with animals is potentially a person-centred approach to strengthen a sense of self-identity [49].

A person-centred approach to care is about staff recognising each resident in the care home as a unique individual with likes and dislikes, understanding his/her life story, and appreciating the resident’s preferences in order to offer activities that match his/her interests and capacities [93, 94]. The overwhelming view of the staff in Fossey & Lawrence’s ([37], p.250) study was that involving animals should be “…facilitated to ensure resident’s’ choices and preferences were respected,” yet the evidence suggests that in practice, the attitudes of staff towards animals were likely to determine whether they were considered as part of person-centred care. Additionally, the willingness of care home managers to support resident pets or visiting animals was a critical factor. These findings are supported by Buist et al.’s research on the implementation of green care farm characteristics in long-term care settings which found that staff flexibility and ability to deliver person-centred care, management commitment and vision, and flexible approaches to risk and safety facilitated change and innovation [95]. Notably, Fossey & Lawrence ([37], p.322) observed that staff often presented health and safety concerns as “a default response that negated the need to consider the topic further.” The extent to which health and safety risks were experienced as a perceived or actual barrier is also likely to be closely related to a care home’s approach to person-centred care. According to Ettelt et al.’s typology of care home approaches, in the task-oriented, risk averse care homes, person-centred care happened at the ‘periphery’, with managers highlighting the tension between person-centred care and risk management [96]. For Fox & Ray ([76], p.212), risk reduction is paramount when residents are viewed as a “homogenous vulnerable group requiring protection from harm” and, in the words of Freedman et al. ([38], p.1976), that is because “risk reduction is valued above identity.”

Person-centred care should be an important influence when developing a policy for resident and visiting animals [97]. Given that the evidence suggests that interacting with animals is more than simply an activity that residents do and is important for ‘who they are’ [38], care homes should think carefully about the animal interactions that best match individual residents’ needs and respect individual choice and autonomy. Arguably, the presence of resident pets and communal animals offered residents the possibility of spontaneous interactions on their own terms, whereas interactions with visiting animals were more likely to be structured and time-bounded. While these visits were a highlight for some, they were, for others, ‘another activity’ that had been organised for them by management [52]. This reinforces how a sense of agency was important for some residents and where possible care homes should aim to facilitate participation in animal visits as a ‘meaningful choice’ for residents. Visits from therapy animals for residents with dementia tended to be structured, as in one study where dog visits were prescribed with each referral made by a nurse, and individualised for each resident with a specific purpose such as increase alertness or decrease anxiety [47,48,49]. The dog handlers indicated that by knowing the residents they aimed to tailor each visit to the individual and respond to the residents’ emotions and feelings. The importance of ‘learning to read the residents’ signals’ was observed by one dog handler [39], and demonstrated that the dog handlers sought to support residents with dementia to exercise agency [98]. The challenge for care home staff is how best to balance supporting residents with dementia to exercise agency and motivate them to engage with animal therapy [85].

Previous systematic reviews of animals in the residential care setting have identified the promise of animal-assisted interventions but have largely focussed on quantifiable outcomes and have focussed on specific populations. Whilst most highlight the lack of rigorous studies, the reviews suggest there is weak evidence that targeted animal -assisted therapies might be effective for older adults with dementia in improving social functioning [15, 18, 19], depression [20, 23] and agitation [16, 20, 99]. Borgi et al. (2018) reviewed the evidence of dog-assisted interventions on older adults with depression, including those in care homes [21]. Despite considerable heterogeneity amongst the studies, the authors concluded that dog assisted interventions of at least four weeks or more, had beneficial effects on depressive symptoms for those living in the community as well as those in residential care, and for those with cognitive impairment of any level, in line with findings from previous reviews [23, 100], and further supported by Jain et al. (2019) [22]. The review by Jain et al., included four qualitative studies, from which the authors drew three themes: dogs visiting the home served as ‘transitional objects’ supplementing otherwise missing interaction, dogs were ‘therapeutic’ in creating a good moment and reducing stress, and lastly the importance of the care home environment in facilitating the most out of dog-assisted interventions [22]. However, there is a recognised dearth of qualitative reviews on the experience of pets and animals in this setting [22, 101]. It seems intuitive that we need to understand the value of pets and animals to residents in long-term care before we try to assess if they are ‘effective’. The qualitative synthesis and conceptual model from our review brings new insight into this area.

Strengths and limitations of review

A strength of this review is that it followed best practice guidelines for both quantitative and qualitative syntheses and was informed by stakeholders. We searched widely for relevant literature and did not limit by date or language. It is also the first systematic review to bring together qualitative and qualitative evidence on human-animal interactions in the long-term care setting: being inclusive of all type of animal interactions, from resident animals and pets through to structured therapeutic visits. It highlights the value of qualitative research in capturing the richness, diversity and similarities in resident experiences of animal interactions. Six of the included papers were drawn from two studies [47,48,49,50,51,52] and Table 2 shows that these papers made similar contributions to the thematic synthesis, apart from one [50].

The potential for animals to elicit negative responses from residents received little attention in the studies. The benefits of animals for residents in long-term care has “almost universal acceptance” ([102], p.151) with the public eager to consume the ‘feel-good’ stories published by the mass media [103]. However, this may present researchers with a ‘double-edged sword’ ([77], p.7), in that a ready audience for their findings may exert a subtle pressure to report positive findings and overlook the”…need to document what might seem the obvious” ([102], p.151). Whilst there were examples of residents becoming over attached or possessive with the animals, and others simply disinterested, the focus was more on the potential benefits. Future research should address this need to document some of the resident-specific challenges of introducing animals into long-term care settings.

The potential discrepancies observed across the qualitative and quantitative syntheses may in part be due to the appropriateness and sensitivity of the outcome measures being used in the trials which may not reflect the value or impact that are important to the participants. They also may be a reflection that the sample sizes in the trial evidence were too small to detect meaningful differences.

There also is a possibility that the difference is explained by the wide and diverse nature of the human-animal interactions that have been explored and assessed across studies to date. The type of animal, the nature of the interaction (individual vs communal, therapy vs activity), and the personality and history of the resident, inevitably leads to a heterogeneity of experience and impact which is difficult to capture and unlikely to be uniform. This is acknowledged, to some extent, in the conceptual model (Fig. 2) which proposes that the experiences and impacts of the human-animal interaction are nuanced by the type of animal interaction which may vary and change across time. Arguably, more research attention should focus on understanding the strengths and limitations of the different types of human-animal interactions themselves.

There was little in the studies about how living in, or visiting, care homes impacted the health and wellbeing of the animals. There is a paucity of research on the possible ill-effects of human interaction on animals [103] and the dominant utilitarian approach in research focuses on ‘what can animals do for us?’ ([75], p.39). Staff raised the welfare of resident pets and the potential for neglect [37] but did not discuss any other aspects of animal wellbeing. Gorman ([104], p.318) counsels against assuming that animals do not receive any benefits from their encounters with humans in a care setting, by arguing that it “…gives rise to the view of animals as passive and lacking agency, simply receiving human action.” He believes that human-animal relations have the potential to be ‘reciprocally beneficial’. The potential for animals being active partners within the relationship and receiving benefit is likely to vary according to the type of animal and its role in the care setting and in this review, some animals were pets belonging to individuals living in the care homes, others were communal animals and part of the care home environment, and others were visiting with either volunteers or trained animal handlers. Arguably, the chickens in the HENPOWER project were less likely to have a mutually beneficial relationship with residents in that they were part of the homes’ outdoor spaces and residents were less likely to interact with the chickens themselves [104]. Understanding more about the experience of animals is essential to ensure that they too have opportunities to mutually benefit from interacting with care home residents. There was also very little in the studies that considered the perceptions and experiences of family members, and only a few studies examined the experiences and perspectives of the volunteers [46] and the dog handlers [39, 48, 50].

The review is limited by the quality of the included quantitative studies. In particular, many of the randomised trials were small, and of short duration, with little or no follow-up. In addition, for the majority of the trials both the residents and researchers were aware of group allocation. Another limitation of the quantitative evidence is the appropriateness of outcome measures, which may not reflect the value or impact that are important to the participants. The qualitative research included in the review was generally of higher quality.

Conclusion

This systematic review demonstrates that animals can significantly impact the health and wellbeing of some care home residents. Residents had meaningful relationships with pets, resident and visiting animals, which were experienced as embodied and emotionally supportive, and from which they derived pleasure and comfort. Interacting with animals offered residents a way to maintain a sense of self in the care homes, and with support, residents with dementia could also express their identities. Facilitating residents to interact with animals as part of person-centred care may also help residents to feel ‘at home’ in the care home, which, according to Cooney, [93] is associated with ‘continuity’, ‘preserving personal identity’, ‘belonging’ and ‘being active and working.’ How care homes facilitate the human-animal interaction, whether it is pets living in the home with their owners or communal animals, or animals as visitors—designated as ‘activity’ or ‘therapy’, the critical factor is that residents’ agency is recognised and respected. Where animals are not a viable option for care homes, robotic animals that mimic ‘living’ animals and respond to human touch, might be a viable alternative [33]. Any care homes considering resident or visiting pets and animals should have an animal policy in place, as recommended by the UK animal charity Blue Cross [105], and establish connections with local vet services. In conclusion, care homes should consider carefully how to encourage human-animal interactions as it will impact the daily functioning and wellbeing of those living, working and visiting long-term care environments.

Availability of data and materials

This file has five tables referenced in the main paper and shows the search summary, the ENTREQ statement, the quality appraisal of the qualitative studies using the Wallace criteria, the Risk of Bias, and illustrative quotations from the primary studies according to analytical themes. This file has two figures referenced in the main paper and shows the thematic network and the metanalyses showing effects of animal assisted intervention on outcomes of depression, anxiety, agitation and quality of life. There are two files, one for the search strategy for MEDLINE and one for PRISMA.

References

  1. Office for National Statistics. Changes in the Older Resident Care Home Population between 2001 and 2011. London: ONS; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Care home and non-care home populations used in the Deaths involving COVID-19 in the care sector article, England and Wales [Internet]. Office of National Statistics. 2020 [cited 04/04/2022]. Available from: https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/birthsdeathsandmarriages/deaths/adhocs/12215carehomeandnoncarehomepopulationsusedinthedeathsinvolvingcovid19inthecaresectorarticleenglandandwales.

  3. Harris-Kojetin L, Sengupta M, Park-Lee E, Valverde R, Caffrey C, Rome V, et al. Long-Term Care Providers and services users in the United States: data from the National Study of Long-Term Care Providers, 2013-2014. Vital Health Stat 3. 2016;38:x-xii–1-105.

    Google Scholar 

  4. The European Commission and the Social Protection Committee. 2021 Long-Term Care Report: Trends, challenges and opportunities in an ageing society. Luxembourg; 2021.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Gordon AL, Franklin M, Bradshaw L, Logan P, Elliott R, Gladman JR. Health status of UK care home residents: a cohort study. Age Ageing. 2014;43(1):97–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Gaugler JE, Yu F, Davila HW, Shippee T. Alzheimer’s disease and nursing homes. Health Aff (Millwood). 2014;33(4):650–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Sullivan GJ, Williams C. Older Adult Transitions into Long-Term Care: A Meta-Synthesis. J Gerontol Nurs. 2017;43(3):41–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. de Medeiros MMD, Carletti TM, Magno MB, Maia LC, Cavalcanti YW, Rodrigues-Garcia RCM. Does the institutionalization influence elderly’s quality of life? A systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Geriatr. 2020;20(1):44.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Gardiner C, Laud P, Heaton T, Gott M. What is the prevalence of loneliness amongst older people living in residential and nursing care homes? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Age Ageing. 2020;49(5):748–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Simard J, Volicer L. Loneliness and Isolation in Long-term Care and the COVID-19 Pandemic. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2020;21(7):966–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Velayudhan L, Aarsland D, Ballard C. Mental health of people living with dementia in care homes during COVID-19 pandemic. Int Psychogeriatr. 2020;32(10):1253–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Johansson K, Borell L, Rosenberg L. Qualities of the environment that support a sense of home and belonging in nursing homes for older people. Ageing Soc. 2022;42:157–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Rijnaard MD, van Hoof J, Janssen BM, Verbeek H, Pocornie W, Eijkelenboom A, et al. The Factors Influencing the Sense of Home in Nursing Homes: A Systematic Review from the Perspective of Residents. J Aging Res. 2016;2016:6143645.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Bert F, Gualano MR, Camussi E, Pieve G, Voglino G, Siliquini R. Animal assisted intervention: A systematic review of benefits and risks. Eur J Integr Med. 2016;8(5):695–706.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Barker SB, Wolen AR. The benefits of human-companion animal interaction: a review. J Vet Med Educ. 2008;35(4):487–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Bernabei V, De Ronchi D, La Ferla T, Moretti F, Tonelli L, Ferrari B, et al. Animal-assisted interventions for elderly patients affected by dementia or psychiatric disorders: a review. J Psychiatr Res. 2013;47(6):762–73.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Filan SL, Llewellyn-Jones RH. Animal-assisted therapy for dementia: a review of the literature. Int Psychogeriatr. 2006;18(4):597–611.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Virues-Ortega J, Pastor-Barriuso R, Castellote JM, Poblacion A, e Pedro-Cuesta J. Effect of animal-assisted therapy on the psychological and functional status of elderly populations and patients with psychiatric disorders: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology Review. 2012;6(2):197– 221.

  19. Yakimicki ML, Edwards NE, Richards E, Beck AM. Animal-Assisted Intervention and Dementia: A Systematic Review. Clin Nurs Res. 2019;28(1):9–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Hu M, Zhang P, Leng M, Li C, Chen L. Animal-assisted intervention for individuals with cognitive impairment: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials and quasi-randomized controlled trials. Psychiatry Res. 2018;260:418–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Borgi M, Collacchi B, Giuliani A, Cirulli F. Dog Visiting Programs for Managing Depressive Symptoms in Older Adults: A Meta-Analysis. Gerontologist. 2020;60(1):e66–75.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Jain B, Syed S, Hafford-Letchfield T, O’Farrell-Pearce S. Dog-assisted interventions and outcomes for older adults in residential long-term care facilities: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Older People Nurs. 2020;15(3): e12320.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Lundqvist M, Carlsson P, Sjodahl R, Theodorsson E, Levin LA. Patient benefit of dog-assisted interventions in health care: a systematic review. BMC Complement Altern Med. 2017;17(1):358.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Higgins JPT, & Green, S. . Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.1.0 [updated March 2011]: The Cochrane Collboration; 2011. Retrieved from http://handbook.cochrane.org.

  25. Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, Mulrow C, Gotzsche PC, Ioannidis JP, et al. The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate healthcare interventions: explanation and elaboration. BMJ. 2009;339:b2700.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Wallace A, Croucher K, Quilgars D, Baldwin S. Meeting the challenge: developing systematic reviewing in social policy. Policy Polit. 2004;32(4):455–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Higgins JP, Altman DG, Gotzsche PC, Juni P, Moher D, Oxman AD, et al. The Cochrane Collaboration’s tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ. 2011;343:d5928.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Thomas J, Harden A. Methods for the thematic synthesis of qualitative research in systematic reviews. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2008;8:45.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Atrride-Stirling J. Thematic networks: an analytic tool for qualitative research. Qual Res. 2001;1(3):385–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Tong A, Flemming K, McInnes E, Oliver S, Craig J. Enhancing transparency in reporting the synthesis of qualitative research: ENTREQ. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2012;12:181.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. DerSimonian R, Laird N. Meta-analysis in clinical trials revisited. Contemp Clin Trials. 2015;45(Pt A):139–45.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Thompson Coon J, Gwernan-Jones R, Garside R, Nunns M, Shaw L, Melendez-Torres GJ, et al. Developing methods for the overarching synthesis of quantitative and qualitative evidence: The interweave synthesis approach. Res Synth Methods. 2020;11(4):507–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Abbott R, Orr N, McGill P, Whear R, Bethel A, Garside R, et al. How do “robopets” impact the health and well-being of residents in care homes? A systematic review of qualitative and quantitative evidence. Int J Older People Nurs. 2019;14(3):e12239.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Casey J, Csiernik R, Knezevic D, Ebear J. The impact of animal-assisted intervention on staff in a seniors residential care facility. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction. 2018:1238-1248.

  35. Cook G, Cook M, Thynne E, Chandler C. An evaluation of 'HENPOWER:' Improving wellbeing & social captial in care settings. Newcastle upon Tyne: Northumbria University; 2013.

  36. Dookie AL. Perspectives of Animal Assisted Activities on Empowerment, Self-Esteem and Communication With Caregivers on Elders in Retirement Homes. Act Adapt Aging. 2013;37(3):189–212.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Fossey J, Lawrence V. Staff views on the involvement of animals in care home life: An exploratory study. J Sociol Soc Welf. 2013;40(4):305–33.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Freedman S, Paramova P, Senior V. ‘It gives you more to life, it’s something new every day’: an Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis of wellbeing in older care home residents who keep a personal pet. Ageing Soc. 2021;41(9):1961–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Gundersen ED, Johannessen B. What motivates arrangements of dog visits in nursing homes? Experiences by dog handlers and nurses. Complement Ther Clin Pract. 2018;31:104–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Kawamura N, Niiyama M, Niiyama H. Animal-assisted activity: experiences of institutionalized Japanese older adults. J Psychosoc Nurs Ment Health Serv. 2009;47(1):41–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Kendziorski SA, Treacy CL. The effects of animal-assisted therapy at a long-term care facility. Masters dissertation Ann Arbor: Grand Valley State University; 1999.

  42. Kongable LG, Stolley JM, Buckwalter KC. Pet therapy for Alzheimer’s patients: a survey. J Long Term Care Adm. 1990;18(3):17–21.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Pitheckoff N, McLaughlin SJ, de Medeiros K. "Calm . . . Satisfied . . . Comforting": The Experience and Meaning of Rabbit-Assisted Activities for Older Adults. J Appl Gerontol. 2018;37(12):1564-1575.

  44. Pooley S. Maintaining affectional bonds. PSIGE Newsletter. 2007;101:5–12.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Roenke L, Mulligan S. The therapeutic value of the human-animal connection. Occup Ther Health Care. 1998;11(2):27–43.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Savishinsky J. Pets and family relationships among nursing home residents. Marriage Fam Rev. 1985;8(3–4):109–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Swall A, Ebbeskog B, Lundh Hagelin C, Fagerberg I. Can therapy dogs evoke awareness of one’s past and present life in persons with Alzheimer’s disease? Int J Old People Nurs. 2015;10(2):84–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Swall A, Ebbeskog B, Lundh Hagelin C, Fagerberg I. “Bringing respite in the burden of illness” - dog handlers’ experience of visiting older persons with dementia together with a therapy dog. J Clin Nurs. 2016;25(15–16):2223–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Swall A, Ebbeskog B, Hagelin CL, Fagerberg I. Stepping out of the shadows of Alzheimer’s disease: a phenomenological hermeneutic study of older people with Alzheimer’s disease caring for a therapy dog. Int J Qual Stud Health Well-Being. 2017;12(1):8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Swall A, Craftman Å, Grundberg Å, Wiklund E, Väliaho N, Hagelin CL. Dog handlers’ experiences of therapy dogs’ impact on life near death for persons with dementia. Int J Palliat Nurs. 2019;25(2):65–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Wong G. The experience of animal therapy in residential aged care in New Zealand : a narrative analysis. Doctoral dissertation. Massey University; 2015.

  52. Wong G, Breheny M. The experience of animal therapy in residential aged care in New Zealand: a narrative analysis. Ageing Soc. 2021;41:2641–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Zando KM. "Paw by paw:” animal assisted therapy with nursing home residents diagnosed with Alzheimer’s Disease. Masters dissertation. Southern Connecticut University; 2017.

  54. Andrysco RM. A study of ethologic and therapeutic factors of pet-facilitated therapy in a retirement-nursing community. Dissertation Abstracts International. 1982;43(1-B):290.

  55. Banks MR, Willoughby LM, Banks WA. Animal-assisted therapy and loneliness in nursing homes: use of robotic versus living dogs. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2008;9(3):173–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Banks MR, Banks WA. The effects of animal-assisted therapy on loneliness in an elderly population in long-term care facilities. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2002;57(7):M428–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Bumsted DL. The effect of pet therapy on the self-care of the elderly. Masters dissertation. Ann Arbor: Southern Connecticut State University; 1988.

  58. Briones MA, Pardo-Garcia I, Escribano-Sotos F. Effectiveness of a Dog-Assisted Therapy Program to Enhance Quality of Life in Institutionalized Dementia Patients. Clin Nurs Res. 2021;30(1):89-97.

  59. Colombo G, Buono MD, Smania K, Raviola R, De Leo D. Pet therapy and institutionalized elderly: a study on 144 cognitively unimpaired subjects. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2006;42(2):207–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Friedmann E, Galik E, Thomas SA, Hall PS, Chung SY, McCune S. Evaluation of a pet-assisted living intervention for improving functional status in assisted living residents with mild to moderate cognitive impairment: a pilot study. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 2015;30(3):276–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Greer KL, Pustay KA, Zaun TC, Coppens P. A comparison of the effects of toys versus live animals on the communication of patients with dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Clin Gerontol. 2002;24(3–4):157–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Johnson WL. The effects of pet encounter therapy on mood states and social facilitation in nursing home residents. Diss Abstr Int. 1997;58(4 B):2124.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Le Roux MC, Kemp R. Effect of a companion dog on depression and anxiety levels of elderly residents in a long-term care facility. Psychogeriatrics. 2009;9(1):23–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Olsen C, Pedersen I, Bergland A, Enders-Slegers MJ, Patil G, Ihlebaek C. Effect of animal-assisted interventions on depression, agitation and quality of life in nursing home residents suffering from cognitive impairment or dementia: a cluster randomized controlled trial. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2016;31(12):1312–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Panzer-Koplow SL. Effects of animal-assisted therapy on depression and morale among nursing home residents. Diss Abstr Int. 2000;61(1 B):544.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Pope WS, Hunt C, Ellison K. Animal assisted therapy for elderly residents of a skilled nursing facility. J Nurs Educ Pract. 2016;6(9):56–62.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Thodberg K, Sørensen L, Christensen J, Poulsen P, Houbak B, Damgaard V, et al. Therapeutic effects of dog visits in nursing homes for the elderly. Psychogeriatrics [Internet]. 2016; 16(5):[289–97 pp.]. Available from: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/o/cochrane/clcentral/articles/458/CN-01210458/frame.html.

  68. Thodberg K, Sorensen LU, Videbech PB, Poulsen PH, Houbak B, Damgaard V, et al. Behavioral Responses of Nursing Home Residents to Visits From a Person with a Dog, a Robot Seal or a Toy Cat. Anthrozoos. 2016;29(1):107–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Travers C, Perkins J, Rand J, Bartlett H, Morton J. An evaluation of dog-assisted therapy for residents of aged care facilities with dementia. Anthrozoos. 2013;26(2):213–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Valenti Soler M, Aguera-Ortiz L, Olazaran Rodriguez J, Mendoza Rebolledo C, Perez Munoz A, Rodriguez Perez I, et al. Social robots in advanced dementia. Front Aging Neurosci. 2015;7:133.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Wall MJ. The effects of companion animal visitation on mood state and level of speech activity of nursing home residents. SAN DIEGO: California School OF Professional Psychology; 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  72. Zulauf JB. The effectiveness of a pet therapy visitation program on the institutionalized elderly [Ph.D.]. Ann Arbor: University of Montana; 1987.

    Google Scholar 

  73. Banks MR. The effects of animal-assisted therapy on loneliness in an elderly population in long-term care facilities. Doctoral dissertation. Louisiana State University Health Sciences Center; 1998.

  74. Normann HK, Asplund K, Norberg A. Episodes of lucidity in people with severe dementia as narrated by formal carers. J Adv Nurs. 1998;28(6):1295–300.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Hatch A. The view from all fours: A look at an animal-assisted activity program from the animals’ perspective. Anthrozoos. 2007;20(1):37–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Fox M, Ray M. No pets allowed? Companion animals, older people and residential care. Med Humanit. 2019;45(2):211–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Serpell J, McCune S, Gee N, Griffin JA. Current challenges to research on animal-assisted interventions. Appl Dev Sci. 2017;21(3):223–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Serpell JA. Anthropomorphism and anthropomorphic selection–beyond the “cute response.” Soc Anim. 2003;11(1):83–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Beetz AM. Theories and possible processes of action in animal assisted interventions. Appl Dev Sci. 2017;21(2):139–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Charles N. “Animals Just Love You as You Are”: Experiencing Kinship across the Species Barrier. Sociology. 2014;48(4):715–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Paddock K, Brown Wilson C, Walshe C, Todd C. Care Home Life and Identity: A Qualitative Case Study. Gerontologist. 2019;59(4):655–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Johnson RA, Bibbo J. Relocation decisions and constructing the meaning of home: a phenomenological study of the transition into a nursing home. J Aging Stud. 2014;30:56–63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Tatzer VC. Narratives-in-action of people with moderate to severe dementia in long-term care: Understanding the link between occupation and identity. J Occup Sci. 2019;26(2):245–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  84. Milte R, Shulver W, Killington M, Bradley C, Ratcliffe J, Crotty M. Quality in residential care from the perspective of people living with dementia: The importance of personhood. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2016;63:9–17.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Strick K, Abbott R, Thompson Coon J, Garside R. Meta-ethnography of the purpose of meaningful occupation for people living with dementia. Int J Older People Nurs. 2021;16(5):e12391.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Bush E. The use of human touch to improve the well-being of older adults. A holistic nursing intervention. J Holist Nurs. 2001;19(3):256–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Gleeson M, Timmins F. The use of touch to enhance nursing care of older person in long-term mental health care facilities. J Psychiatr Ment Health Nurs. 2004;11(5):541–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Cekaite A, Goodwin MH. Touch and Social Interaction. Annu Rev Anthropol. 2021;50:203–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Douglas C. A World of Touch in a No-Touch Pandemic Living with Dementia in a Care Facility during COVID-19. Anthropol Action. 2021;28(1):8–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Taylor JS. On recognition, caring, and dementia. Med Anthropol Q. 2008;22(4):313–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Kontos P. Embodied selfhood in Alzheimer’s disease: Rethinking person-centred care. Dementia (London). 2005;4(4):553–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Downs M. Embodiment: the implications for living well with dementia. Dementia (London). 2013;12(3):368–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Cooney A. “Finding home”: a grounded theory on how older people “find home” in long-term care settings. Int J Older People Nurs. 2012;7(3):188–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Brown M, Mitchell B, Boyd A, Tolson D. Meaningful activity in advanced dementia. Nurs Older People. 2020. https://doi.org/10.7748/nop.2020.e1171.

  95. Buist Y, Verbeek H, de Boer B, de Bruin SR. Innovating dementia care; implementing characteristics of green care farms in other long-term care settings. Int Psychogeriatr. 2018;30(7):1057–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Ettelt S, Williams l, Damant J, Perkins M, Wiitemberg R. What kind of home is your care home? A typology of personalised care provided in residential and nursing homes. Ageing & Society. 2022;5:993–1013.

  97. Smith R, Johnson J, Rolph S. People, pets and care homes: a story of ambivalence. Qual Ageing Old Adults. 2011;12(4):217–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  98. Boyle G. Recognising the agency of people with dementia. Disability & Society. 2014;29(7):1130–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  99. Charry-Sanchez JD, Pradilla I, Talero-Gutierrez C. Animal-assisted therapy in adults: A systematic review. Complement Ther Clin Pract. 2018;32:169–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Perkins J, Bartlett H, Travers C, Rand J. Dog-assisted therapy for older people with dementia: a review. Australas J Ageing. 2008;27(4):177–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Ebener J, Oh H. A Review of Animal-Assisted Interventions in Long-Term Care Facilities. Activities, Adaption & Ageing. 2017;41(2):107–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  102. Kazdin AE. Strategies to improve the evidence of animal-assisted interventions. Appl Dev Sci. 2017;21(2):150–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  103. Fine AH, Beck AM, Ng Z. The State of Animal-Assisted Interventions: Addressing the Contemporary Issues that will Shape the Future. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019;16(20):3997.

  104. Gorman R. What’s in it for the animals? Symbiotically considering “therapeutic” human-animal relations within spaces and practices of care farming. Med Humanit. 2019;45(3):313–25.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  105. Blue Cross. All care homes should have a pet policy says Blue Cross 2021 [Available from: https://www.bluecross.org.uk/news/all-care-homes-should-have-a-pet-policy-says-blue-cross.

Download references

Acknowledgements

Our thanks to Dr Zoe Belshaw for her constructive comments on the draft, and to George Coxon and Jan Hazell for their input as project advisers.

Funding

This work was supported by University of Exeter Medical School, University of Exeter; The National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Applied Research Collaboration South West Peninsula. Views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the University of Exeter or the NIHR.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JTC, RA, AB, RW, RG & NO conceived of the project; AB carried out the systematic search of the literature; RA, NO, SP & RW selected the studies. RA & NO evaluated the methodological quality and extracted the data of the included studies. RA & NO drafted the manuscript and all authors revised and commented on the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Noreen Orr.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file1: FileS1.

Search strategy for MEDLINE. Table S1. Search Summary. Table S2. ENTREQ Statement (Enhancing the transparency in reporting the synthesis of qualitative research). Table S3. Quality Appraisal of Included Qualitative Studies. Table S4. Quality Appraisal of Randomised Trials. Table S5. Illustrative Quotations from Primary Studies according to Analytical Themes. Figure S1. Thematic Network. Figure S2. Meta-analyses showing effects of animal assisted intervention on outcomes of depression, anxiety, agitation and quality of life. File S2. PRISMA checklist.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Orr, N., Abbott, R., Bethel, A. et al. What are the effects of animals on the health and wellbeing of residents in care homes? A systematic review of the qualitative and quantitative evidence. BMC Geriatr 23, 170 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-023-03834-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-023-03834-0

Keywords