Skip to main content

Protocol for a randomized controlled trial of mindfulness-based stress reduction to improve attentional control in older adults (HealthyAgers trial)

Abstract

Background

Mindfulness meditation is a form of mind–body intervention that has increasing scientific support for its ability to reduce age-related declines in cognitive functioning, improve affective health, and strengthen the neural circuitry supporting improved cognitive and affective health. However, the majority of existent studies have been pilot investigations with small sample sizes, limited follow-up data, and a lack of attention to expectancy effects. Here, we present the study design of a Phase I/II, efficacy trial—HealthyAgers trial—that examines the benefits of a manualized mindfulness-based stress reduction program in improving attentional control and reducing mind-wandering in older adults.

Methods

One hundred fifty older adults (ages 65–85 years) will be randomized into one of two groups: an eight-week mindfulness program or an eight-week, placebo-controlled, lifestyle education program. Behavioral and neuroimaging assessments are conducted before and after the training. Participants are then invited to booster sessions once every three months for a period of 12 months with post-intervention follow-up assessments conducted at 6-months and 12-months. The primary outcomes for the study are behavioral measures of attentional control and mind-wandering. Additional, secondary outcomes include network strength in an a priori defined neuromarker of attentional control, fluid and everyday cognition, emotion regulation strategy use, and markers of inflammation.

Discussion

This study will establish the efficacy of a group-based, low-cost mind–body intervention for the inter-related facets of attentional control and mind-wandering in older adults. Strengths of this study include a well-designed, placebo-controlled comparison group, use of web/mobile application to track study adherence, and longitudinal follow-up.

Trial registration

Clinicaltrials.gov (#NCT03626532). Registered August 4, 2018.

Peer Review reports

Background

Older adults are a rapidly growing sector of the U.S. population as adults over the age of 65 years are projected to almost double from 56 million in 2020 (17% of the population) to 95 million in 2060 (23% of the population) [1]. Maintaining cognitive faculties is an important public health goal as cognitive functioning is highly related to well-established markers of physical health, like systemic inflammation [2, 3], and plays a critical role in activities that help maintain independent living, like driving [4], and in emotion dysregulation [5,6,7]. Thus, cognitive functioning has direct implicatons for the well-being and satisfaction of older adults [8]. Age-related declines in cognitive functioning have been noted across the domains of processing speed, working memory, executive control of attention, cognitive control, and episodic memory [9,10,11], with declines in some domains of cognition beginning as early as the mid-30s [12]. Moreover, behavioral shifts in measures of cognitive functioning are also accompanied by concomitant shifts in the functional and structural integrity of the aging brain with older adults showing neural dedifferentiation—the reduced recruitment of specialized canonical networks and nodes during cognitive tasks with increasing age [13, 14]. These age-related declines in cognitive functioning, both behavioral and neural representations, are thought to be undergirded by the increase in task irrelevant observations with advancing age. Older adults demonstrate a failure to maintain attentional sets, to computationally prioritize task-relevant information, and to limit attentional overprocessing of task-irrelevant information, resulting in notable declines in higher-order control operations [15,16,17].

However, despite a more nuanced and in-depth understanding of the cognitive and neural changes associated with aging, the health field’s capacity to translate these findings into pragmatic, efficacious remediation methods remains limited. Cognitive training programs show modest improvements in overall cognition with limited benefits to tasks of everyday functioning [18, 19]. Although studies of strategy-specific cognitive training are able to identify factors that may mediate the impact of the intervention for changes in cognition, there have been limited far-reaching transfer effects [20, 21]. Comparatively, multi-modal interventions, combining across various training modalities [22, 23] show the most promising effects. However, in these studies, it has been difficult to isolate the mechanisms of action. Thus, the scientific premise of the current study is based on an urgent need to explore alternative training programs that provide theoretical promise for reducing age-related cognitive decline, particulary in attentional overprocessing of task-irrelevant information. This could then have broad transfer effects on emergent cognitive operations of cognitive control, emotion regulation, and for domains of cognitive functioning that directly impact everyday functioning, like driving.

Mindfulness training, traditionally offered as an eight-week program called the mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) [24], has gained increased popularity for its potential to improve cognition [25, 26], ameliorate symptoms of depression and anxiety [27], alter perceived and objective markers of stress [28], enhance emotion regulation [29], and alter the neural circuitry supporting cognitive and emotional functioning [30, 31]. Mindfulness, as defined in the contemporary sciences, is the practice of purposefully directing attention, in a non-judgmental way, to observe the unfolding of each moment as it takes place [32]. As such, the various components of attention, including the fluctuations in attention—quantified via the propensity to engage in mind-wandering—are being investigated for their malleability as a function of engagement in mindfulness practices. In our recent scoping review of the contemplative sciences literature examining the impact of mindfulness meditation for components of attention, we found the most robust evidence for mindfulness training to impact executive control of attention [33]. Moreover, results of a pilot, randomized controlled trial in our lab also provided evidence for mindfulness training to reduce off-task thinking, or mind-wandering quantified using both self-reported thought probes and response time variability in healthy older adults [34]. These reductions in mind-wandering propensity, replicated by other clinical trials (see Feruglio [35] for a systematic review), provide preliminary support that training in mindfulness meditation may strengthen the goal maintenance system, enhancing one’s ability to prioritize task-relevant representations, and to filter out irrelevant, internal thoughts or external stimuili in the environment.

Taken together, there is a growing recognition for the potential for mindfulness practices to improve attentional control—the biasing of task-relevant representations at the temporal, spatial, and response stages. However, this literature is still in its infancy, particularly in the evidence supporting mindfulness meditation as a tool for reducing age-related cognitive decline. The majority of studies, including our own pilot clinical trial, have had limited follow-up data, have relied on self-reported data for quantifying adherence to study materials, and did not include broad measures of transfer effects. This Phase I/II efficacy trial—HealthyAgers—aims to build on the existing literature by examining the immediate and longitudinal impact, relative to an active control group, of an eight-week mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) program in improving attentional control and reducing mind-wandering in older adults. In addition, the control group in the study—Lifestyle Education (LifeEd) program—has been carefully designed to match the training group for non-specific factors that may influence the outcomes of interest. Participants in both groups will engage in a booster session every three months across the 12-month period following the intervention. Importantly, we have designed an in-house web/mobile application—HealthyAgers—to measure engagement with study materials for both groups to allow for careful assessments of dose–response relationships. We hypothesize that mindfulness training, compared to lifestyle education training, will improve attentional control and reduce mind-wandering. Secondary measures of this study will also assess the broad impact of mindfulness meditation for functional connectivity, fluid and everyday cognition, emotion regulation, and systemic inflammation. We hypothesize that mindfulness training will additionally result in an improvement in these secondary measures of healthy aging. The results of this study will establish the efficacy of this increasingly popular, low-cost intervention for older adults with regard to cognitive, affective, and neural health.

Methods/design

Study design

The proposed study is a single-blind, randomized controlled trial (RCT) designed to examine the differential impact of mindfulness training compared to a lifestyle education group (LifeEd) on behavioral and neural measures of attentional control and mind-wandering. Secondary assessments involve measures of fluid cognition, everyday cognition, emotion regulation, and systemic markers of inflammation. One hundred fifty older adults (65–85 years old) are being recruited for the study and randomized to either an eight-week MBSR group or an eight-week LifeEd control group. Behavioral, neuroimaging, and inflammation data will be conducted at baseline prior to randomization and again at the conclusion of the eight-week intervention. Further, participants will be followed for an additional 12 months, during which time booster sessions will be provided once every three months, with cognitive and affective assessments performed at 6-month and 12-month follow-ups. Neuroimaging and inflammation data will only be collected at the 12-month follow-up. Figure 1 illustrates a timeline of study participation.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flowchart of the HealthyAgers Trial

Setting

Assessments and the intervention will be conducted in the Psychology Building at The Ohio State University. The Department of Psychology houses individual neuropsychological testing rooms to allow for the convenient administration of neuropsychological and emotional regulation measures. All structural and functional MRI data will be collected at the Center for Cognitive and Behavioral Brain Imaging housed in the Department of Psychology at Ohio State. Prior to the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic, all intervention sessions were provided in the group therapy spaces housed in the Department of Psychology. These spaces were specifically designed for offering mindfulness and LifeEd sessions. Intervention sessions have been shifted to a virtual format following the COVID-19 pandemic, but assessment sessions continue to be collected in-person.

Participant eligibility

Participants will only be included in the study if they satisfy the following criteria: aged between 65–85 years, corrected (far) visual acuity of 20/40 or better, have adequate hearing for assessment purposes, and have self-reported proficiency in spoken English and in reading comprehension. Additionally, we will only include participants if they are able to attend the majority of eight-week sessions and attend all assessment sessions, have a self-reported interest in engaging in mind–body interventions, can commit to a 30-min homework practice five times per week, and have the ability to engage in light stretching and toning exercises.

Additionally, participants will only be included in the study if they are classified as cognitively normal or having mild cognitive impairment (MCI) based on a battery of neuropsychological measures employed to screen individuals with dementia [36, 37]. The screening battery includes the following measures and seven dependent variables used for exclusionary determinations: 1) total recall and 2) delayed recall from the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test—Revised ([38], 3) total score on the Block Design and 4) total score on Digit Span subtests from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV [39], 5) total score on the FAS Verbal Fluency Task [40], 6) total score on the Boston Naming Test [41], and 7) perseverative errors on the computerized version of the Wisconsin Card Sort Test [42]. The WAIS-IV subtests are completed between the immediate and delayed recall tasks of the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test. Descriptions of each of these measures and details on their scoring and norms are provided in Supplementary Materials. Participants will be excluded from the study due to having impairment levels consistent with dementia based on one of the following criteria: 1) if at least one normed score in both the memory and non-memory domains is < -2 S.D.s below the mean (i.e., T < 30), 2) if the average of their normed scores on all seven neuropsychological measures is < -2 S.D.s (i.e., average T < 30), or 3) if the participant reports less than intact function on all items of the Lawton Instrumental Activities of Daily Living [43] (< 8/8 points).

Participants will be classified as having MCI if they do not meet the criteria for dementia but have at least one normed score < -1.5 S.D.s below the mean (i.e., T < 35) or if the average of their seven normed score is < -1.5 S.D.s (i.e., average T < 35). Participants will be classified as cognitively normal if all of their normed scores are within 1.5 S.D.s of the mean and they receive a full score on the Lawton Instrumental Activities of Daily Living. These categorizations are consistent with previous clinical definitions of MCI and dementia [44, 45] and with previous research in MCI and dementia classification [36, 37]. However, an important limitation with this approach is that, although our categorization of MCI may be consistent with clinical conceptualization, it has been demonstrated that a significant minority of healthy older adults will have at least one “impaired” score < -1.5 S.D.s from the mean when given a neuropsychological battery [46, 47]. This has been of particular concern when trying to characterize MCI [48, 49]. In the event that the normed scores for a participant are not able to be computed due to errors in a given test’s administration, the participant will be assessed for cognitive impairment using an electronic version of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment [50] with its provided cut-off scores (26/30 or higher denoting normal cognition).

Other reasons for exclusion of potential participants include if they have been previously diagnosed with a neurological disorder, an inflammatory disorder, or a terminal illness. Participants with a history of psychotic disorders, substance abuse, or a prior learning disability will also be excluded. Additionally, participants diagnosed with a psychiatric disorder in the last two years by a mental health professional and those scoring greater than 19 on the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale [51] will be excluded from the study. Participants currently taking medications belonging to any of the following drug classes will be excluded: sedatives, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, benzodiazepines, barbiturates, sedative-hypnotics, anti-inflammatories, chemotherapies, and any drugs altering brain function or enhancing cognitive performance. Anyone who has engaged in formal mindfulness training within the past year will also be ineligible or with current regular practice of meditation—defined as practicing any kind of meditation practice for greater than 10 min per day—will be ineligible. As the study involves participants engaging with practices on a web/mobile platform, participants will also be excluded from the study if they do not have access to the internet. Participants without access to computers but with internet access will be provided mini-iPads.

Sample size

In our pilot study [34], we found a partial eta square of 0.088 for the repeated-measures ANOVA examining the effect of mindfulness on Go/No-Go performance as a function of working memory index. Based on an alpha of 0.05, a total sample size of 103 would be needed to yield an estimated power greater than 0.80. Given the attrition observed in our pilot study (~ 20%), the 12-month duration of the proposed intervention, and multiple assessment sessions, we will recruit at least 130 participants to account for a potential 25% drop-out rate. Power analysis for the imaging data was based on the parameter estimates in the Kilpatrick study [52] examining the impact of mindfulness training on functional connectivity of resting-state networks. The resulting effect sizes were d = 1.29 and d = 0.99 for auditory/salience regions To be conservative, we used the effect size of d = 0.99. To detect an effect of this size with 90% power, we would need 23 subjects per group for a total of 46 people. A study by Kilpatrick and colleagues [52] compared their mindfulness intervention against a wait-list control group. It is likely that the parameter estimates of their study include variance that may be accounted for by non-specific factors. In the present study, our mindfulness training group will be compared with an active control group that will control for many of the non-specific factors. Thus, assuming an effect size of d = 0.76, we plan on collecting MRI data on 75 participants to yield an estimated power greater than 0.90.

Study procedures

Participants are being drawn from the greater central Ohio area. Recruitment for the study started in August 2018 and is currently ongoing. We are implementing a multi-medium recruitment strategy, advertising for the study using both printed and online communications, in-person recruitment, community outreach, and other opportunities aimed at our targeted demographic. Table 1 provides a list of our primary recruitment sources for the study. Participants interested in the study will either complete a phone screen or an online survey that is designed to collect basic demographic information, current medications, and history of physical as well as psychological health. Participants who meet basic inclusionary criteria are then invited to the Clinical Neuroscience Laboratory at The Ohio State University to complete an in-person screening session. Those meeting the study criteria as described above are invited for additional behavioral and imaging-based assessments. Table 2 lists the measures administered at pre-training, post-training, and at 6-month and 12-month follow-up sessions. Participants are also given a Qualtrics link with the following survey measures at baseline, pre-training, post-training, and the two follow-up sessions: the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale [53], the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale [54], the Perceived Stress Scale [55], the World Health Organization Quality of Life – BREF [56], the Beck Anxiety Inventory [57], the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire [58], the Coronavirus Anxiety Scale [59], Psychological Stress Associated with the COVID-19 Crisis [60], and the Everyday Discrimination Scale [61].

Table 1 List of primary recruitment sources being employed for the study
Table 2 Measures being administered across study visits for the HealthyAgers Trial

Primary outcome

The primary outcomes for this study are changes in measures of attentional control and mind-wandering during tasks of sustained attention. To quantify both attentional control and mind-wandering the following measures are being administered:

Go/No-Go

In this task, participants are presented with one of two visual stimuli (“X” and “M”, or “Z” and “/”) for 750 ms followed by an inter-trial-interval of 750 ms. They are asked to press the corresponding key on frequent Go trials and to withhold responses on the less frequent (occurring 10% of the time) No-Go trials signaled by an auditory tone. Participants complete six blocks of this task with each block containing 54 Go trials, six No-Go trials, and three mind-wandering probes. The quasi-randomly presented mind-wandering probes ask participants to categorize their immediately preceding thought as: 1) “On-task”, 2) “thinking about performance on the task”, or 3) “thinking about personal worries, day-dreaming, fantasizing or just lost in thought.” The primary outcome variable for attentional control will be the signal detection sensitivity index (dL). The dL provides a measure of task performance and will be calculated using the formula for logistic distributions: dL = ln{[H(1-FA)]/[(1-H)FA]}, where H refers to hit rates for go trials and FA refers to false alarms on no-go trials. The primary outcome variable for mind-wandering will be the proportion of off-task probes endorsed by the participant. In addition, we will compute response-time variability—the trial-by-trial fluctuation in reaction time—as an indirect marker of mind-wandering.

Conner’s continuous performance test

In this task, participants are presented with a series of letters displayed one at a time on the screen and are instructed to press the space bar as quickly as possible for all letters (target), except for the letter ‘X’ (non-target). Participants are instructed to withhold their responses during ‘X’ letter (non-target) trials. Participants complete six blocks of 60 trials each in which letters are presented for 250 ms and inter-stimulus intervals are 1, 2, or 4 s in duration. Participants’ ability to differentiate between non-targets and targets is reflected by detectability (d’) scores and will be the primary outcome assessing attentional control. Participant d’ scores are reverse-scored such that higher T-scores reflect worse performance (i.e., poorer discrimination between targets and non-targets). Response time variability (RT variability), an index of mind-wandering for this task, is a measure of response speed consistency and will be calculated as the standard deviation of each participant’s response time (ms) on correct trials. Higher RT variability scores is indicative of varying attentional states and will be employed as an indirect index of mind-wandering in this task.

The efficacy of the intervention for sustained attention will be determined by improved performance on both measures. Similarly, we will conclude that mindfulness training causally impacts mind-wandering if there are reductions in both self-reported mind-wandering (i.e., on embedded probes) and in response time variability.

Secondary outcomes

Secondary outcomes in this study include neuroimaging assessments, measures of fluid cognition, everyday cognition, and emotion regulation strategy use, and systemic markers of inflammation.

Neuroimaging assessment

Functional MRI (fMRI) and structural MRI data will be collected at pre-training, post-training, and at 12-month follow-up. The primary dependent variable from the neuroimaging data is the network strength in a connectome-based predictive model of sustained attention (saCPM; [62]). Figure 2 presents the functional neuroanatomy of the high attention model (includes functional connections that positively predict attentional control) and the low attention model (includes functional connections that negatively predict attentional control). The saCPM model, combining the functional connections of the high and low attention models, will be employed in the current study. Network strength in this whole-brain, functional connectivity-based model of attentional control predicts performance on variegated tasks of attentional control [63,64,65], generalizes to predict performance across young adults [66], older adults [65], and individuals with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder [62], and, critically, shows responsiveness to change following methylphenidate treatment for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder [63]. Participants will perform the gradual-onset continuous performance task (gradCPT; [62, 67, 68]) while in the scanner. In this task, participants view circular images of city and mountain scenes. Participants are asked to respond to the frequently occurring city scenes (~ 90% of images) and to withhold responses to mountain scenes. Participants will complete two separate runs with each run lasting 13 min and 44 s. Neuroimaging data will be checked for quality control and preprocessed using our lab’s established pipelines [69]. The preprocessed 4D data for each participant will be parcellated into 268 contiguous and functionally defined regions using a parcellation scheme covering the cortex, subcortex, and cerebellum [70]. The connections between each pair of these nodes, defined as edges, will be computed by correlating the time-course of activity between the nodes. This will result in a 268X268 functional connectivity matrix with cells of the matrix representing the magnitude of these edges or correlations. Using the network mask of the saCPM neuromarker [63], which has been validated in our prior work with older adults [71], we will compute the network strength of the marker for each participant at pre-training, post-training, and at 12-month follow-up.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Functional neuroanatomy of the sustained attention connectome-based predictive model (saCPM). A Presents the involvement of the ten canonical networks in the high attention model with edges representing functional connections that positively predict attentional control. Ribbons in the ring plot visualization are proportional to the representation of each network in the saCPM model. The matrix presented on the bottom shows the relative contribution of each network to the saCPM model adjusted using the formula described in Greene et al., 2019. Panel B highlights the network localization of the low attentonl model with edges representing functional connections that negatively predict attentional control. The matrix on the bottom shows the relative contribution of each network in the saCPM model. Chord diagrams were generated using the software Flourish (v7.0.0, https://app.flourish.studio)

NIH cognitive toolbox

The cognition battery of the NIH Toolbox was designed as part of the NIH Blueprint for Neuroscience Research [72] to assess cognitive functioning across multiple domains in a standardized format. The battery is designed for use with participants between the ages of 3–85 with alternate forms available for longitudinal assessments. All tasks will be completed using fifth generation iPads with set-up and administration standardized across all participants. We will employ the fully corrected fluid cognition T-scores that norm for age, gender, education, and race/ethnicity for the following five tasks: Pattern Comparison Processing Speed, List Sort Working Memory, Picture Sequence Memory, Dimensional Change Card Sort, and Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention. Please see Supplementary Materials for a detailed description of each of these tasks.

Neuropsychological Assessment Battery (NAB) driving scenes

Driving is an important functional domain that plays a critical role in the independence of older adults. To assess the impact of the intervention on functional ability, participants are being administered the Driving Scenes subtest of the NAB as a proxy measure of everyday functional ability. This measure has been shown to serve as a valid proxy for real-world driving abilities [73], it can identify at-risk drivers [74], and it is associated with global cognitive impairment in older adults with and without cognitive impairment [75]. Participants are presented with one driving scene for 30 s. This is followed by the presentation of a new scene for 60 and up to 120 s. Participants are asked to identify elements of the new scene that may be new, diferrent, or missing compared to the first scene. The main dependent variable from this task is the total number of correctly identified elements as adjusted for age, sex, and education [76].

Emotion regulation task

Participants complete an adapted version of the Emotion Regulation Choice Task originally developed by Sheppes and colleagues [77]. Participants are shown a series of 24 negatively valenced images and are instructed to regulate their emotions to each image using one of two emotion regulation strategies, namely acceptance or suppression. Twelve of the trials consist of low-intensity images and the remaining 12 trials consist of high-intensity images. All images were taken from the International Affective Picture System [78]. Prior to completing the task, participants undergo a brief training in which they read about each emotion regulation strategy and are asked to verbalize to the experimenter how they are using these strategies during the six practice trials. The order for which emotion regulation strategy is learned first will be counterbalanced across participants. Broadly, acceptance is defined as allowing one’s feelings to come and go without trying to control or avoid them, and suppression is defined as pushing one’s feelings down or out of one’s mind. Following the practice trials, participants proceed to the 24 task trials. Overall acceptance use will be calculated as the percentage of total trials (24 trials) in which the participant chooses to use acceptance to regulate their emotions. Acceptance use will be quantified as the percentage of low- and high-intensity images (12 trials each), respectively, in which the participant chooses to use acceptance. Scores can range between 0 and 100 percent, and higher scores reflect a greater preference for acceptance.

Systemic markers of inflammation

Blood samples will be collected between 8:00 am-10:00 am by a trained phlebotomist at three timepoints: pre-training, post-training, and at the 12-month follow-up session. Following standardized protocols, and implementing same control for each participant at all time points, the frozen samples for each participant will be assayed for the inflammatory markers at the same time. Various peripheral inflammatory markers exist which may be investigated for their association with cognition; however, there is strong support for C-reactive protein, interleukin-6, and tumour necrosis factor alpha to be consistently associated with cognitive outcomes in older adults [79, 80]. Additionally, these inflammatory markers appear to be reduced following mindfulness meditation interventions in a dose–response relationship [81,82,83]—an effect with several biologically plausible mechanisms [84,85,86]. This relationship appears particularly robust in older adults [87]. Although some previous studies have examined composite scores from multiple inflammatory markers, these composites are not standardized across studies and biomarkers are implicated in unique physiological processes [88,89,90]. Therefore, we will examine peripheral inflammation via these three markers independently.

Randomization

Eligible and consenting participants will be randomized after completing baseline assessments to one of two groups: MBSR or LifeEd. A random permuted-block design (blocks of size 2 and 4) will be used, and the randomization with an allocation ration of 1:1 will be stratified by sex assigned at birth (male, female), cognitive status (cognitively normal, MCI), and MRI eligibility (eligible, not eligible) to ensure balance across these important factors. The biostatistician on the team, R.A., prepared and maintains the randomization sequence for the eight strata. The project coordinator has been given concealed envelopes and only after a participant completes all pre-assessment sessions and is deemed eligible for the study does the project coordinator open the envelope from the respective stratum and assign the participant to either the MBSR or the LifeEd group. The project coordinator, the study facilitators and supervisors, and the study biostatistician will be the only members of the team who will be unblinded to group assignment. All personnel conducting assessments, behavioral or neuroimaging, will remain blind to group assignment.

Study interventions

Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) program

The MBSR program being offered in this study is modeled after the eight-week program developed and popularized by Jon Kabat-Zinn to reduce pain and stress [24]. The MBSR program is offered in a group setting wherein participants meet with trained facilitators once a week for two and a half hours each (except for the first class which is three hours) along with a four-hour retreat offered on a weekend (between weeks six and seven)—thus resulting in 24.5 h of face-to-face training. In these classes, facilitators cover a combination of experiential practices, didactics and teachings on the topics of mindfulness, and mindful movement exercises. These practices and teachings all center on cultivating present moment awareness in a framework that promotes acceptance and non-judgment. Moreover, the participants are provided with several opportunities to engage in small and large group-based discussions that offer a platform for social support, validation, and shared experiences. Supplementary Table 1 provides the agenda for each of the eight sessions of the MBSR program as well as the practices taught during the retreat. In addition to these classes, all participants are invited to engage in 30 min of formal homework practices, delivered through our web/mobile-based, in-house application that was named after the study—HealthyAgers. Working with a team of inter-disciplinary experts, including certified mindfulness experts, psychologists, wellness coaches, and computer programmers, we have carefully designed this iOS/Android mobile/web application to deliver and track guided meditation practices. This application will allow us to collect objective practice data on all our participants with the application providing information on minutes practiced, the frequency of engagement in mindfulness practices per week, and additional meta-data, including preferred practices, time of day preference, etc. After the eight weeks of the intervention, participants will be invited to continue engagement with mindfulness practices on their own, five times per week, for 12 months with two-hour, group-based booster sessions provided by the study facilitators every three months. Prior to the pandemic, all intervention sessions were provided in our group psychotherapy rooms housed in the Department of Psychology. Sessions shifted to the Zoom platform in March 2020 and continue to be offered virtually.

Lifestyle Education (LifeEd) program

The LifeEd program is an active control group against which we will compare the effects of the MBSR program. Participants are provided with psychoeducation on topics related to healthy living and wellness for older adults. The series includes didactics covering the latest evidence-based science on: sedentary behavior, physical activity, cognitively stimulating activities, social support, sleep, stress management, nutrition, and hydration. This program is designed to control the many active ingredients of a group-based psychotherapeutic program, like MBSR. As such, it is matched for setting (group), number and training of facilitators, amount of time receiving in-person instruction, and amount of assigned homework time. In addition, to ensure that it is matched with respect to engagement in experiential exercises, participants are introduced to light stretching and toning exercises during the two-and-a-half-hour class sessions. These stretching and toning exercises were designed by a certified yoga instructor with expertise in working with older adults. Supplementary Table 1 also provides detailed agendas for all eight sessions of the LifeEd program. Moreover, participants in the LifeEd program also have access to the web/mobile application—HealthyAgers—with participants in this group engaging in the following combination of homework practices: light stretching and toning exercises three times/week, reading two popular press articles and answering questions related to them once a week, and watching one to two videos and answering related questions once a week. All homework materials are standardized to take ~ 30 min with all readings and videos delivered through the in-house application. Finally, like the MBSR program, participants in the LifeEd group are also being provided booster sessions once every three months with the encouragement to continue completing homework practices five times per week. Prior to the pandemic this group also met in-person with trained facilitators and transitioned to Zoom sessions starting in March 2020 because of the COVID-19 pandemic.

Facilitation of MBSR and LifeEd groups and treatment fidelity

Advanced graduate students in clinical psychology facilitate both the MBSR and LifeEd groups. The study PI, RSP, and her team’s mindfulness instructor, CR, provide 25 h of training for facilitating MBSR groups and another 25 h of training for facilitating LifeEd groups to advanced students with prior training in individual and group psychotherapy. Detailed training manuals, outlining the session agendas and transcripts, are provided to the students. Additionally, throughout the facilitation of these programs, new facilitators receive weekly group supervision (1.5 h.) and individual supervision (30 min). In accordance with the guidelines proposed by the Treatment Fidelity Workshop of the NIH [91] and making adaptations consistent with published guidelines for establishing fidelity for mindfulness-based interventions, [92] we will assess the fidelity for both protocols on the five identified components: design, training, delivery, receipt, and enactment. Table 3 provides details on the fidelity assessments being conducted for the current study.

Table 3 Fidelity components being assessed for the current study

Assessing and maximizing adherence

Our lab has successfully conducted two RCTs on mindfulness training, and for both those studies, we have had adherence rates of ~ 80% (Whitmoyer et al., 2020; Schirda et al., 2020). In the current study, we will implement a number of strategies that have been successful in the past to promote adherence to the intervention as well as the assessment sessions. First, as both groups in the study will engage in training that promotes and discusses healthy aging, advertising the study as a trial on mind–body interventions for healthy aging will help recruit individuals with a genuine interest in making these changes. Second, detailing the study requirements in the recruitment materials, particularly the 30 min of daily practice for eight weeks, appears to have a positive influence on adherence to homework practices and retention rates. All our advertisement materials detail this time commitment on behalf of the participants. Third, once participants are enrolled in the study, weekly attendance at the online sessions is being monitored by course facilitators and communicated to the study coordinator. For every missed session, the study coordinator connects with the participant, and either schedules them for an individual appointment with the course facilitator or sends them a summary from the missed session. For participants who wish to not continue with the intervention, every attempt will be made to complete the assessment sessions.

Data management and analysis

All study data (with the exception of blood samples) are being stored in the PI’s laboratory located in the Psychology Building at Ohio State. The laboratory is only accessible to approved personnel. Hard data files (e.g., consent forms, neuropsychological assessment data) are stored in locked cabinets inside the laboratory, whereas electronic data files (e.g., behavioral data, self-report questionnaires, neuroimaging data) are stored on a password-protected laboratory server. De-identified participant blood samples are stored at the Clinical Research Center at The Ohio State University.

Cognitive screening data will be coded by two independent research assistants in the lab with coder disagreements resolved by an experienced research assistant. Participants’ performance scores from the Conner’s Continuous Performance Test and the National Institute of Health Toolbox Cognition battery are automatically generated from each program and will then organized on the laboratory server via manual procedures and/or custom lab scripts. Participant’s behavioral data from the Go/No-Go, Emotion Regulation Choice, Gradual-Onset Continuous Performance Task, and the N-back task will be organized and coded by two independent laboratory members using custom scripts. Neuroimaging data quality will be assessed using a validated quality control pipeline [69] prior to conducting pre-processing. Pre-processing of the neuroimaging data will be conducted by either a postdoctoral researcher or an advanced graduate student in the lab with extensive training in neuroimaging analyses. All remaining processing and coding of neuroimaging data will be completed by two independent graduate students/postdoctoral researchers. Markers of inflammation from participant’s blood samples will be processed by the Clinical Research Center at The Ohio State University.

After preprocessing of behavioral and neuroimaging data, all data will first be tested for normality, outliers, and errors. We will then compute summary statistics and compare groups’ baseline demographic and clinical characteristics; although randomization should balance the groups with respect to these characteristics, with a finite sample size, there may be some residual imbalance. Particular attention will be paid to characteristics that may be associated with outcomes (e.g., education, depression scores) as chance imbalance in these characteristics may confound the intervention effects. All statistical analyses conducted as part of the trial will be intent-to-treat. The primary outcome variables for this study are indices of attentional control and mind-wandering. To examine the immediate effects of mindfulness training, we will use a linear mixed model to account for the within-subject correlation arising from measuring each subject at two timepoints. Group (MBSR, LifeEd), Time (pre and post-training), and the Group X Time interaction will be defined as fixed effects, with covariates, when applicable (e.g., those identified as randomly differing between groups) included as fixed effects. Each participant’s intercept will be included as a random effect. To determine the maintenance effects of mindfulness meditation, we will construct a separate mixed linear model examining the long-term efficacy of mindfulness meditation with Time (pre-training, 6-month follow-up, and 12-month follow-up), Group (MBSR, LifeEd), and the Group X Time interaction effects. Secondary factors of functional connectivity, fluid and everyday cognition, emotion dysregulation and inflammation will also be examined by constructing linear mixed models.

Adverse event reporting

Adverse events are being graded using the common grading scale proposed by the National Institutes of Health: (a) No adverse event (event is within normal limits or not clinically significant), (b) Mild adverse event that does not require treatment, (c) Moderate adverse event that was resolved with treatment, (d) Severe adverse event that resulted in an inability to carry on normal activities and required professional medical attention, (e) Life threatening or disabling adverse event, and (f) Fatal adverse event. All study personnel are trained to monitor for adverse events, and, upon notification of an adverse event, the study PI (RSP) determines the relationship of the adverse event(s) to the intervention as unrelated, possibly related, probably related, or definitely related using the standard criteria for clinical trials. For every adverse event, a written report of the adverse event is submitted to the local IRB within 10 days of the occurrence of the event. The IRB along with the PI then determines the relationship of these events to the study protocol and determines whether the adverse event was related the design of the study. If the adverse event is not related to the design of the study or is a mild adverse event related to the design of the study, these adverse events are documented along with any changes in the protocol that will be implemented to prevent future occurrences. Additionally, the event is reported to the NIH and the Data and Safety Monitor appointed for this study at the bi-annual meeting. If the adverse event is reported to be related to the study and is classified as a moderate adverse event and above, in addition to submitting a report to the IRB, a full report will be prepared and submitted within 10 days of the adverse event to the Data and Safety Monitor. This report will include a full, detailed description of the event as well as the response of the research team. If two participants experience the same adverse event, we will immediately stop recruitment for the study, prepare a report for the IRB and the Data and Safety Monitor, and incorporate any revisions to the study protocol suggested by the monitor. Additionally, annual reports will also be submitted to the IRB indicating the progress of the study, including information on enrollments, withdrawals and completions, and the occurrence of adverse events.

Dissemination

Informed consent documents for this study include a specific statement regarding the registration of this clinical trial as well as availability of results of this clinical trial after study completion on NIH’s clinical trials platform. Per NIH Policy on the Dissemination of NIH-Funded Clinical Trial Information (NOT-OD-16–149), we have registered the proposed study on NIH’s clinical trial platform (clinicaltrials.gov # #NCT03626532) prior to enrolling the first participant. After study completion, we will also promptly report results of the clinical trial on NIH’s clinical trial platform to provide easy access to our study findings to other researchers, health care providers, and patient communities. These summary results will be submitted to NIH’s clinical trial platform no later than one year after the primary completion date. We will also be proactive, yet cautious, in our dissemination of the results of these findings through public presentations, peer-reviewed journal publications as well as communicating the results of these studies to our study participants. After publication of primary and seconday manuscripts from this dataset, all data will be de-identified, and made publicly available.

Ethical considerations

The Ohio State Institutional Review Board has granted approval to conduct this study (IRB #2017H0223). All participants provide written informed consent and all study personnel have received training in the ethical conduct of human subject research.

Discussion

This ongoing Phase I/II randomized controlled trial (HealthyAgers) is designed to evaluate the efficacy of mindfulness-based stress reduction, compared to a carefully designed, active control group (Lifestyle Education), in improving attentional control, reducing mind-wandering, and strengthening the functional connections subserving attentional processing in older adults. With the provision of quarterly booster sessions and the availability of a web-based and mobile application designed to promote adherence to mindfulness practices, this study will also be able to determine the longitudinal effect of engagement in mindfulness practices for maintenance of attentional benefits. Additionally, with secondary assessments involving global and everyday measures of cognition, emotion regulation, and systemic markers of inflammation, results of this study will also add to our understanding of whether mindfulness training has the potential to improve cognitive and affective health of older adults. This systematically designed trial of mindfulness meditation with its inclusion of an active control group, assessment of theoretically-driven behavioral and neural metrics of attentional control, and longitudinal follow-up will make a significant and nuanced contribution to our understanding of whether and how engagement in mindfulness meditation promotes healthy aging.

Mindfulness meditation is becoming increasingly popular because of its promise to influence several integrated metrics of overall health—attentional control [33], emotion dysregulation [93, 94], perceived and objective markers of stress [95, 96], loneliness [97, 98], systemic markers of peripheral inflammation [82], and relationship quality [99]. The adaptation of mindfulness programs to a variety of settings was also catapulted by the increasing rise of stress and mental health problems during the COVID-19 pandemic. However, many systematic and scoping reviews of the mindfulness literature have highlighted the heterogeneity of findings within contemplative sciences and have called for a more systematic and controlled study of mindfulness meditation [33, 100]. Our scoping review [33], examining the effects of mindfulness on attentional control, surveyed mindfulness training studies through the lens of five study design issues essential for establishing causality [101, 102]: 1) randomization of participants to groups; 2) inclusion of an active control group; 3) explicit attention to the reduction of demand characteristics; 4) detailed discussion of content of the intervention and control groups; and 5) implementation of study reporting guidelines (such as CONSORT). Of the 57 studies included in the review, only four met all five essential criteria—thus necessitating further and more rigorous study of mindfulness meditation for attentional control. Additionally, ten studies included an active control group and, of those, only five explicitly discussed the procedural steps and the efforts made to reduce demand characteristics in study participants.

The three most common control groups included across the studies on mindfulness meditation involved relaxation controls, education groups, and book reading groups. Importantly, factors of social support, such as collaborative discussion within group formats and engagement in cognitively stimulating activities, were highlighted as key non-specific factors influencing attention. Our control group—the Lifestyle Education Control Group—has been matched with respect to multiple metrics. Format of delivery: both protocols were delivered in-person before the COVID-19 pandemic, and we shifted to a virtual format (after the pandemic), offering classes synchronously via the Zoom platform with participants recruited in groups of six to eight for peer support. Timing: each of the eight weekly classes (plus the retreats) is matched for timing. Content: both protocols have a mix of didactics, experiential practices, social support, and community building. Peer support: qualitative analysis of focus group discussions on mindfulness-based interventions[103,104,105] highlight the need for participants to connect and communicate with others engaging in the interventions. Both groups have built-in dyadic and small-group discussions on topics such as weekly materials, challenges with homework practices, and overall learning in the program. Homework assignment: both programs require engagement in daily homework assignments through our in-house application for 30 min/day for five days/week.

In our study, in addition to the inclusion of this active control group, we have implemented several procedural steps to ensure that perceived expectations— participants’ beliefs regarding the efficacy of the intervention for outcome measures [33, 101]—are minimally influenced by study design, experimenter bias, and advertising materials. Inclusion of an active control group that is matched with respect to timing, duration, content, and engagement allows for the study to be advertised using common elements for the two groups rather than an explicit focus on mindfulness meditation. All our recruitment materials—print-ads, radio ads, flyers, presentations—describe the study as examining the effect of two mind–body interventions on healthy aging. Employing the definition provided by the National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health, “mind–body practices focus on the interactions among the brain, mind, body, and behavior, with the intent to use the mind to affect physical functioning and promote health,” both our recruitment materials and the content for the active control group included elements of mind–body practices. The key difference between the two groups is the active engagement with mindfulness practices as opposed to a focus on psychoeducation in the control group. In addition to reducing demand characteristics through blinded assessors, unbiased recruitment materials, and the use of an active control group can minimize their effects, it is also essential to measure these perceived expectations. In the present study, participants in both groups are completing measures of perceived expectations at the end of the first intervention session and after the respective groups have been introduced, with the eventual goal of examining differential expectations between the two groups. Finally in our attempt to examine the impact of demand characteristics on treatment gains carefully, our primary analyses will also include an association of perceived expectations with any observed gains on outcome variables.

An additional strength of this study is the inclusion of an a priori validated neuromarker of sustained attention to examine the changes in functional connectivity following training in mindfulness meditation. Although fMRI is frequently employed to examine the effects of behavioral interventions, the precise mapping of neuroimaging features relevant to target behavior is often missing. In our prior work [65], we have established the validity of this neuromarker of sustained attention—saCPM— in older adults. In this study, network strength in this established biomarker will be examined for training-related gains, with network strength in the high attention model expected to increase as a function of training and network strength in the low attention model expected to decrease. Given the centrality of attentional control to the processes of mind-wandering and emotion regulation, we hypothesize that changes in this neuromarker will mediate mindfulness-related training gains on these metrics.

In conclusion, the HealthyAgers trial will provide data on the efficacy of mindfulness meditation to improve older adults’ attentional control abilities, reduce mind-wandering, and strengthen the neural circuitry supporting attention. By virtue of enhancing the ability to sustain attention, mindfulness meditation may also improve general cognition, increase the use of acceptance-based emotion regulation strategies, and reduce overall levels of systemic inflammation. Given the aging of the baby boomer generation and the impact of reduced attentional control for activities of daily living and quality of life, training in mindfulness meditation may, in fact, be one pathway to promote healthy aging.

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable. This is a protocol paper so there is no data employed in the manuscript.

Abbreviations

ANOVA:

Analysis of variance

Conner’s CPT:

Conner’s Continuous Performance Test-3 

CONSORT:

Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials

ERC:

Emotion Regulation Choice task

fMRI:

functional magnetic resonance imaging

grad-CPT:

Gradual-onset continuous performance task

IRB:

Institutional Review Board

LifeEd:

Lifestyle education control group

MBSR:

Mindfulness-based stress reduction

MCI:

Mild cognitive impairment

MoCA:

Montreal Cognitive Assessment

MRI:

Magnetic resonance imaging

NAB Driving Scenes:

Neuropsychological Assessment Battery Driving Scenes test

NIH:

National Institutes of Health

NIHTB Cognition:

National Institute of Health Toolbox Cognition Battery

PI:

Principal Investigator

RCT:

Randomized controlled trial

saCPM:

Sustained attention connectome-based predictive model

WAIS-IV:

Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV

References

  1. United States Census Bureau. International Database: World Population Estimates and Projections. https://www.census.gov/content/dam/Census/library/publications/2020/demo/p25-1144.pdf.

  2. Yaffe K, Lindquist K, Penninx BW, Simonsick EM, Pahor M, Kritchevsky S, et al. Inflammatory markers and cognition in well-functioning African-American and white elders. Neurology. 2003;61:76–80.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Weaver JD, Huang M-H, Albert M, Harris T, Rowe JW, Seeman TE. Interleukin-6 and risk of cognitive decline MacArthur studies of successful aging. Neurology. 2002;59:371–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. De Raedt R, Ponjaert-Kristoffersen I. The relationship between cognitive/neuropsychological factors and car driving performance in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2000;48:1664–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Mather M, Carstensen LL. Aging and motivated cognition: the positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9:496–502.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Mather M, Knight M. Goal-directed memory: the role of cognitive control in older adults’ emotional memory. Psychol Aging. 2005;20:554–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Urry HL, Gross JJ. Emotion regulation in older age. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2010;19:352–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Nashiro K, Sakaki M, Mather M. Age differences in brain activity during emotion processing: reflections of age-related decline or increased emotion regulation? Gerontology. 2012;58:156–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Glisky EL. Changes in Cognitive Function in Human Aging. In: Riddle DR, editor. Brain aging: models, methods, and mechanisms. Boca Raton (FL): CRC Press/Taylor & Francis; 2007. p. 3–20.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Goh JO, An Y, Resnick SM. Differential trajectories of age-related changes in components of executive and memory processes. Psychol Aging. 2012;27:707–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Verhaeghen P, Salthouse TA. Meta-analyses of age-cognition relations in adulthood: estimates of linear and nonlinear age effects and structural models. Psychol Bull. 1997;122:231–49.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Park DC, Reuter-Lorenz P. The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009;60:173–96.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Prakash RS, Erickson KI, Colcombe SJ, Kim JS, Voss MW, Kramer AF. Age-related differences in the involvement of the prefrontal cortex in attentional control. Brain Cogn. 2009;71:328–35.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Koen JD, Rugg MD. Neural dedifferentiation in the aging brain. Trends Cogn Sci. 2019;23:547–59.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Hasher L, Zacks RT. Working Memory, Comprehension, and Aging: A Review and a New View. In: Bower GH, editor. Psychology of learning and motivation. Academic Press; 1988. p. 193–225.

    Google Scholar 

  16. De Jong R. Adult age differences in goal activation and goal maintenance. Eur J Cogn Psychol. 2001;1–2:71–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Gazzaley A, Cooney JW, Rissman J, D’Esposito M. Top-down suppression deficit underlies working memory impairment in normal aging. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:1298–300.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Lustig C, Shah P, Seidler R, Reuter-Lorenz PA. Aging, training, and the brain: a review and future directions. Neuropsychol Rev. 2009;19:504–22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Willis SL, Tennstedt SL, Marsiske M, Ball K, Elias J, Koepke KM, et al. Long-term effects of cognitive training on everyday functional outcomes in older adults. JAMA. 2006;296:2805–14.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Dunlosky J, Kubat-Silman AK, Hertzog C. Training monitoring skills improves older adults’ self-paced associative learning. Psychol Aging. 2003;18:340–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Rebok GW, Carlson MC, Langbaum JBS. Training and maintaining memory abilities in healthy older adults: traditional and novel approaches. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2007;62 Spec No 1:53–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Fried LP, Carlson MC, Freedman M, Frick KD, Glass TA, Hill J, et al. A social model for health promotion for an aging population: Initial evidence on the experience corps model. J Urban Health. 2004;81:64–78.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Park DC, Lodi-Smith J, Drew L, Haber S, Hebrank A, Bischof GN, et al. The impact of sustained engagement on cognitive function in older adults the synapse project. Psychol Sci. 2014;25:103–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Kabat-Zinn J. An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: theoretical considerations and preliminary results. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 1982;4:33–47.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Jha AP, Stanley EA, Kiyonaga A, Wong L, Gelfand L. Examining the protective effects of mindfulness training on working memory capacity and affective experience. Emotion. 2010;10:54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Moynihan JA, Chapman BP, Klorman R, Krasner MS, Duberstein PR, Brown KW, et al. Mindfulness-based stress reduction for older adults: effects on executive function, frontal alpha asymmetry and immune function. Neuropsychobiology. 2013;68:34–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Segal ZV, Bieling P, Young T, MacQueen G, Cooke R, Martin L, et al. Antidepressant monotherapy vs sequential pharmacotherapy and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy, or placebo, for relapse prophylaxis in recurrent depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67:1256–64.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Baer RA, Smith GT, Lykins E, Button D, Krietemeyer J, Sauer S, et al. Construct validity of the five facet mindfulness questionnaire in meditating and non-meditating samples. Assessment. 2008;15:329–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Chiesa A, Serretti A. Mindfulness based cognitive therapy for psychiatric disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 2011;187:441–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Allen M, Dietz M, Blair KS, van Beek M, Rees G, Vestergaard-Poulsen P, et al. Cognitive-affective neural plasticity following active-controlled mindfulness intervention. J Neurosci. 2012;32:15601–10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Farb NAS, Anderson AK, Mayberg H, Bean J, McKeon D, Segal ZV. Minding one’s emotions: mindfulness training alters the neural expression of sadness. Emotion. 2010;10:25–33.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Kabat-Zinn J. Wherever you go, there you are: mindfulness meditation in everyday life. New York: Hyperion; 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Prakash RS, Fountain-Zaragoza S, Kramer AF, Samimy S, Wegman J. Mindfulness and attention: current state-of-affairs and future considerations. J Cogn Enhanc. 2020;4:340–67.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Whitmoyer P, Fountain-Zaragoza S, Andridge R, Bredemeier K, Londeree A, Kaye L, et al. Mindfulness training and attentional control in older adults: a randomized controlled trial. Mindfulness. 2020;11:203–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Feruglio S, Matiz A, Pagnoni G, Fabbro F, Crescentini C. The impact of mindfulness meditation on the wandering mind: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;131:313–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Scharre DW, Chang S-I, Murden RA, Lamb J, Beversdorf DQ, Kataki M, et al. Self-administered Gerocognitive Examination (SAGE): a brief cognitive assessment Instrument for mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and early dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2010;24:64–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Scharre DW, Chang SI, Nagaraja HN, Vrettos NE, Bornstein RA. Digitally translated Self-Administered Gerocognitive Examination (eSAGE): relationship with its validated paper version, neuropsychological evaluations, and clinical assessments. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2017;9:44.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Brandt J, Benedict RHB. Hopkins verbal learning test – revised. Administration manual. Lutz: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edition (WAIS–IV). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Spreen O, Benton AL. Neurosensory center comprehensive examination for aphasia. Victoria BC: University of Victoria, Neuropsychology Laboratory; 1977.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Kaplan E, Goodglass H, Weintraub S. The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia, PA: Lea & Fibiger; 1983.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Tien AY, Spevack TV, Jones DW, Pearlson GD, Schlaepfer TE, Strauss ME. Computerized wisconsin card sorting test: comparison with manual administration. Kaohsiung J Med Sci. 1996;12:479–85.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Lawton MP, Brody EM. Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist. 1969;9 3_Part_1:179–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th ed. American Psychiatric Association, Inc.; 1994.

  45. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol. 1999;56:303–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Crawford JR, Garthwaite PH, Gault CB. Estimating the percentage of the population with abnormally low scores (or abnormally large score differences) on standardized neuropsychological test batteries: a generic method with applications. Neuropsychology. 2007;21:419–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Palmer BW, Boone KB, Lesser IM, Wohl MA. Base rates of “Impaired” neuropsychological test performance among healthy older adults. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1998;13:503–11.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Bondi MW, Jak AJ, Delano-Wood L, Jacobson MW, Delis DC, Salmon DP. Neuropsychological contributions to the early identification of Alzheimer’s Disease. Neuropsychol Rev. 2008;18:73–90.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Jak AJ, Urban S, Mccauley A, Bangen KJ, Delano-Wood L, Corey-Bloom J, et al. Profile of hippocampal volumes and stroke risk varies by neuropsychological definition of mild cognitive impairment. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15:890–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53:695–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Radloff LS. The CES-D scale a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1:385–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Kilpatrick LA, Suyenobu BY, Smith SR, Bueller JA, Goodman T, Creswell JD, et al. Impact of mindfulness-based stress reduction training on intrinsic brain connectivity. Neuroimage. 2011;56:290–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Brown KW, Ryan RM. The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;84:822–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2004;26:41–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. 1983;24:385–96.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Whoqol Group. Development of the World Health Organization WHOQOL-BREF quality of life assessment. Psychol Med. 1998;28:551–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer RA. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1988;56:893–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Baer RA, Smith GT, Hopkins J, Krietemeyer J, Toney L. Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment. 2006;13:27–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Lee SA. Coronavirus anxiety scale: a brief mental health screener for COVID-19 related anxiety. Death Stud. 2020;44:393–401.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Adamson MM, Phillips A, Seenivasan S, Martinez J, Grewal H, Kang X, et al. International prevalence and correlates of psychological stress during the global COVID-19 pandemic. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17:9248.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Williams DR, Yu Y, Jackson JS, Anderson NB. Racial differences in physical and mental health: socio-economic status stress and discrimination. J Health Psychol. 1997;2:335–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Rosenberg MD, Finn ES, Scheinost D, Papademetris X, Shen X, Constable RT, et al. A neuromarker of sustained attention from whole-brain functional connectivity. Nat Neurosci. 2016;19:165–71.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Rosenberg MD. Methylphenidate modulates functional network connectivity to enhance attention. J Neurosci. 2016;36:9547–57.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Rosenberg MD, Hsu W-T, Scheinost D, Todd Constable R, Chun MM. Connectome-based models predict separable components of attention in novel individuals. J Cogn Neurosci. 2018;30(2):160–73.

  65. Fountain-Zaragoza S, Samimy S, Rosenberg MD, Prakash RS. Connectome-based models predict attentional control in aging adults. Neuroimage. 2019;186:1–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Jangraw DC, Gonzalez-Castillo J, Handwerker DA, Ghane M, Rosenberg MD, Panwar P, et al. A functional connectivity-based neuromarker of sustained attention generalizes to predict recall in a reading task. Neuroimage. 2018;166:99–109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Esterman M, Noonan SK, Rosenberg M, DeGutis J. In the zone or zoning out? Tracking behavioral and neural fluctuations during sustained attention. Cereb Cortex. 2013;23:2712–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Fortenbaugh FC, Rothlein D, McGlinchey R, DeGutis J, Esterman M. Tracking behavioral and neural fluctuations during sustained attention: a robust replication and extension. Neuroimage. 2018;171:148–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Gbadeyan O, Teng J, Prakash RS. Predicting response time variability from task and resting-state functional connectivity in the aging brain.: connectome-based predictive modeling of response time variability in aging. Neuroimage. 2022;250:118890.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Shen X. Using connectome-based predictive modeling to predict individual behavior from brain connectivity. Nat Protoc. 2017;12:506–18.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Manglani HR, Fountain-Zaragoza S, Shankar A, Nicholas JA, Prakash RS. Employing connectome-based models to predict working memory in multiple sclerosis. Brain Connectivity. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1089/brain.2021.0037.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Weintraub S, Dikmen SS, Heaton RK, Tulsky DS, Zelazo PD, Bauer PJ, et al. Cognition assessment using the NIH Toolbox. Neurology. 2013;80(11 Suppl 3):S54-64.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Brown LB, Stern RA, Cahn-Weiner DA, Rogers B, Messer MA, Lannon MC, et al. Driving Scenes test of the Neuropsychological Assessment Battery (NAB) and on-road driving performance in aging and very mild dementia. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2005;20:209–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Stern RA, Abularach LM, Seichepine DR, Alosco ML, Gavett BE, Tripodis Y. Office-based assessment of at-risk driving in older adults with and without cognitive impairment. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2016;29:352–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Ashendorf L, Alosco ML, Bing-Canar H, Chapman KR, Martin B, Chaisson CE, et al. Clinical utility of select neuropsychological assessment battery tests in predicting functional abilities in Dementia. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2018;33:530–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Stern RA, White T. NAB, Neuropsychological Assessment Battery: attention module stimulus book. Form 2. Lutz: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  77. Sheppes G, Scheibe S, Suri G, Gross JJ. Emotion-regulation choice. Psychol Sci. 2011;22:1391–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN. International affective picture system (IAPS): Instruction manual and affective ratings. Gainsville: University of Florida, Center for Research in Psychophysiology; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  79. Sartori AC, Vance DE, Slater LZ, Crowe M. The impact of inflammation on cognitive function in older adults: implications for healthcare practice and research. J Neurosci Nurs. 2012;44:206–17.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  80. TangestaniFard M, Stough C. A review and hypothesized model of the mechanisms that underpin the relationship between inflammation and cognition in the elderly. Front Aging Neurosci. 2019;11:56.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  81. McClintock AS, Goldberg SB, Coe CL, Zgierska AE. Mindfulness practice predicts interleukin-6 responses to a mindfulness-based alcohol relapse prevention intervention. J Subst Abuse Treat. 2019;105:57–63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  82. Sanada K, Montero-Marin J, Barceló-Soler A, Ikuse D, Ota M, Hirata A, et al. Effects of mindfulness-based interventions on biomarkers and low-grade inflammation in patients with psychiatric disorders: a meta-analytic review. Int J Mol Sci. 2020;21:2484.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Rosenkranz MA, Davidson RJ, Maccoon DG, Sheridan JF, Kalin NH, Lutz A. A comparison of mindfulness-based stress reduction and an active control in modulation of neurogenic inflammation. Brain Behav Immun. 2013;27:174–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Fountain-Zaragoza S, Prakash RS. Mindfulness training for healthy aging: impact on attention, well-being, and inflammation. Front Aging Neurosci. 2017;9:11.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  85. Buric I, Farias M, Jong J, Mee C, Brazil IA. What is the molecular signature of mind–body interventions? A systematic review of gene expression changes induced by meditation and related practices. Front Immunol. 2017;8:670.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  86. Creswell JD, Irwin MR, Burklund LJ, Lieberman MD, Arevalo JMG, Ma J, et al. Mindfulness-based stress reduction training reduces loneliness and pro-inflammatory gene expression in older adults: a small randomized controlled trial. Brain Behav Immun. 2012;26:1095–101.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  87. Villalba DK, Lindsay EK, Marsland AL, Greco CM, Young S, Brown KW, et al. Mindfulness training and systemic low-grade inflammation in stressed community adults: evidence from two randomized controlled trials. PLoS ONE. 2019;14:e0219120.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  88. Beydoun MA, Dore GA, Canas J-A, Liang H, Beydoun HA, Evans MK, et al. Systemic inflammation is associated with longitudinal changes in cognitive performance among urban adults. Front Aging Neurosci. 2018;10:313.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  89. Chi GC, Fitzpatrick AL, Sharma M, Jenny NS, Lopez OL, DeKosky ST. Inflammatory biomarkers predict domain-specific cognitive decline in older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2017;72:796–803.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Walker KA, Gross AL, Moghekar AR, Soldan A, Pettigrew C, Hou X, et al. Association of peripheral inflammatory markers with connectivity in large-scale functional brain networks of non-demented older adults. Brain Behav Immun. 2020;87:388–96.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  91. Bellg AJ, Borrelli B, Resnick B, Hecht J, Minicucci DS, Ory M, et al. Enhancing treatment fidelity in health behavior change studies: best practices and recommendations from the NIH Behavior Change Consortium. Health Psychol. 2004;23:443–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Kechter A, Amaro H, Black DS. Reporting of treatment fidelity in mindfulness-based intervention trials: a review and new tool using NIH behavior change consortium guidelines. Mindfulness (N Y). 2019;10:215–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  93. Prakash RS. Mindfulness meditation: impact on attentional control and emotion dysregulation. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2021;36:1283–90.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Schirda B. Mindfulness training for emotion dysregulation in multiple sclerosis: a pilot randomized controlled trial. Rehabil Psychol. 2020;65:206–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Burton A, Burgess C, Dean S, Koutsopoulou GZ, Hugh-Jones S. How effective are mindfulness-based interventions for reducing stress among healthcare professionals? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Stress Health. 2017;33:3–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Khoury B, Sharma M, Rush SE, Fournier C. Mindfulness-based stress reduction for healthy individuals: a meta-analysis. J Psychosom Res. 2015;78:519–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Lindsay EK, Young S, Brown KW, Smyth JM, Creswell JD. Mindfulness training reduces loneliness and increases social contact in a randomized controlled trial. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2019;116:3488–93.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  98. Teoh SL, Letchumanan V, Lee L-H. Can mindfulness help to alleviate loneliness? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Psychol. 2021;12:633319.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  99. McGill JM, Burke LK, Adler-Baeder F. The dyadic influences of mindfulness on relationship functioning. J Soc Pers Relat. 2020;37:2941–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  100. Verhaeghen P. Mindfulness as attention training: meta-analyses on the links between attention performance and mindfulness interventions, long-term meditation practice, and trait mindfulness. Mindfulness. 2021;12:564–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  101. Boot WR, Simons DJ, Stothart C, Stutts C. The pervasive problem with placebos in psychology: why active control groups are not sufficient to rule out placebo effects. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2013;8:445–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Stothart C, Simons D, Boot W, Kramer A. Is the effect of aerobic exercise on cognition a placebo effect? PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e109557.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  103. Murray G. Online mindfulness-based intervention for late-stage bipolar disorder: pilot evidence for feasibility and effectiveness. J Affect Disord. 2015;178:46–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  104. Todd NJ, Jones SH, Lobban FA. What do service users with bipolar disorder want from a web-based self-management intervention? A qualitative focus group study: development of a web-based intervention for bipolar disorder. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2013;20:531–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Vieten C. The mindful moms training: development of a mindfulness-based intervention to reduce stress and overeating during pregnancy. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2018;18:201.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Jennifer Baryluk for creating the content of the stretching and toning videos being employed in the Lifestyle Education group. The authors would also like to thank Cheryl Rapose for providing her expertise in mindfulness meditation to the current study and for creating the content of the mindfulness vidoes. We would also like to thank Harrison Productions for shooting the videos. The authors would also like to thank the entire Clinical Neuroscience Lab research team for their continued instrumental support during recruitment and data collection.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institute on Aging of the National Institutes of Health (R01AG054427 awarded to RSP) and the National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship to MF. The study protocol has undergone full external peer review by the funding body. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the the National Institute on Aging, the National Institutes of Health, or the National Science Foundation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

R.S.P. designed the study, wrote the protocol paper, and incorporated edits from co-authors, S.F.Z., J.K-G., and R.A. assisted with the design of the study. S.F.Z., M.F., O.G., H.R.M., M.M., E.J.D., A.S., J.T., M.P., R.C acquired the data, wrote sections of the protocol paper, and provided edits to the entire manuscript. All authors have read and approved the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ruchika Shaurya Prakash.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The Ohio State Institutional Review Board has granted approval to conduct this study (IRB #2017H0223). All participants provide written informed consent and all study personnel have received training in the ethical conduct of human subject research.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Prakash, R.S., Fountain-Zaragoza, S., Fisher, M. et al. Protocol for a randomized controlled trial of mindfulness-based stress reduction to improve attentional control in older adults (HealthyAgers trial). BMC Geriatr 22, 666 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-022-03334-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-022-03334-7

Keywords