Skip to main content

Prenatal exposure to the Dutch famine is associated with more self-perceived cognitive problems at 72 years of age

A Correction to this article was published on 10 May 2022

This article has been updated

Abstract

Background

Undernutrition during critical periods of neurodevelopment can hinder the developing brain with lasting negative consequences for brain size, structure and function. In this study, we describe self-perceived cognitive problems of men and women who were born around the time of the Dutch famine of 1944–45.

Methods

We compared self-perceived cognitive problems between men and women who had been exposed to the 1944–45 Dutch famine in late, mid or early gestation and those who were born before or conceived after the famine (and had thus not been exposed prenatally). We included 595 participants aged 71–74 years.

Results

Women who had been exposed to famine in late gestation more often reported cognitive problems compared to those who had not been exposed (OR 2.2 [95% CI 1.1–4.4]), whereas for men, this was the case for those exposed in early gestation (OR 2.3 [0.9–5.5]). Furthermore, men and women exposed in early gestation more often reported consulting a healthcare practitioner for cognitive problems in the past 12 months (OR 3.2 [1.3–8.1]). Especially men exposed in early gestation reported having consulted a healthcare practitioner more often than unexposed men (OR 4.4 [1.2–16.0]).

Conclusions

These findings suggest that prenatal undernutrition does not only have lasting effects on brain size, but also on its function, with more self-perceived cognitive problems at older age, which also require more medical attention. Also, the effects of undernutrition depend on sex and its timing during gestation.

Peer Review reports

Background

The significance of early-life factors in age-related cognitive decline and risk for dementia is becoming increasingly clear from several studies in humans and animals [1,2,3,4]. Adverse events during the prenatal and early postnatal period may directly affect late-life cognitive function and dementia risk by lowering brain and cognitive reserve capacity or by changing the rate at which dementia-related pathologies can accumulate (for instance by limiting the brains’ ability to clear these pathologies) [1, 2]. Indirectly, adverse early-life factors may increase the risk for mediating adverse health outcomes, which in turn have been associated with cognitive decline (e.g. cardiovascular disease) [4,5,6]. Additionally, adverse early-life factors may accelerate the general aging process and thereby accelerate brain aging [2].

Studies of men and women born around the time of the 1944–45 Dutch famine have provided direct evidence in humans that undernutrition during critical periods of prenatal development increases the risk of age-related diseases such as type 2 diabetes and coronary heart disease, as well as overall mortality [5]. Furthermore, men and women exposed to famine in early gestation performed worse on a selective attention task in their late fifties, which may be an early manifestation of accelerated cognitive aging [6]. Because self-reported cognitive problems may be an indicator of cognitive aging and can be a predictor for future dementia [7], we here describe findings of self-reported cognitive problems at a mean age of 72 years as a further investigation of the effects of prenatal undernutrition on future cognitive decline.

Methods

Participants

The Dutch famine birth cohort (DFBC) consists of individuals born alive as term singleton babies in the Wilhelmina Gasthuis (a teaching hospital in Amsterdam, the Netherlands) between November 1, 1943 and February 28, 1947. In 1994, 2155 (89.3%) individuals could be included in the DFBC (Fig. 1). In the current study, 1207 individuals were eligible and invited to participate, of which 595 (49.3%) gave written informed consent and completed the questionnaire. The Medical Ethics Review Committee of the Academic Medical Center concluded that a full review and official approval of this study was not required according to Dutch law.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Derivation of study participants. The questionnaire was sent to all 1207 eligible participants. aThese babies had not been registered in Amsterdam at birth. bThese individuals refused permission to record their address or retracted their permission later. cInvalid home address and could not be reached by phone or no correct phone number and validity of home address unknown. dNo telephone contact established (5 attempts) or contact established but no subsequent reaction. eParticipants were physically or mentally ill, or cognitively impaired and therefore declined participation

Exposure

The Dutch famine was a consequence of events occurring at the end of World War II, this cascade of events has been described in detail elsewhere [5, 6]. Daily food rations dropped steeply, reaching a level below 1000 kcal per person on November 26, 1944 and improved relatively quickly after the liberation of the Netherlands in May 1945. There was a more or less proportionate drop of protein, carbohydrate and fat. In correspondence with previous publications on the DFBC, we considered three different 16-week exposure periods: individuals mainly exposed in late gestation (born between January 7 and April 28, 1945), mid-gestation (born between April 29 and August 18, 1945) or early gestation (born between August 19 and December 8, 1945) [6]. Individuals born in these periods were prenatally exposed to at least 13 weeks of famine during which their mother’s daily food-ration contained on average fewer than 1000 kcal. As children younger than 1 year of age were relatively protected against the famine, both individuals born before (born before January 7, 1945) and conceived after the famine (born after December 8, 1945) were considered unexposed and acted as a control group.

Self-perceived cognitive problems

Self-perceived cognitive problems were assessed with two questions as part of a larger questionnaire (including mental and physical health, daily life functioning, quality of life and stressful life events). One question was selected from The Older Persons and Informal Caregivers Survey – Minimum DataSet (TOPICS-MDS) validated questionnaire (version before 2017) [8]. Participants had to select the answer that best described their health at that time: “1) I have no problems with my memory, attention and thinking; 2) I have some problems with my memory, attention and thinking; 3) I have severe problems with my memory, attention and thinking.” The second question was adapted from the TOPICS-MDS (version 2017): “Did you consult a doctor or other healthcare practitioner for problems with your memory, attention and thinking in the past 12 months?” [8].

Study parameters

Maternal, pregnancy and birth characteristics were collected from participants’ medical birth records. Hospital anxiety and depression scale (HADS) scores [9], highest achieved education and living situation were derived from the current questionnaire. Socioeconomic status (SES) was derived from individuals’ four-digit current postal code as reported in the questionnaire by matching with Statistics Netherlands (CBS) data. For individuals who had participated in the DFBC studies in 2002 or 2008, we retrieved HADS scores and information about diabetes, stroke and cardiovascular health status at a mean age of 58 and/or 63 years.

Statistical analyses

We performed logistic regression analyses to investigate associations between famine exposure during late, mid or early gestation and self-perceived cognitive problems, compared to the control groups (Table 2). For each analysis, we ran a crude model and a model adjusted for covariates (sex, birth weight, education and SES). As we have repeatedly observed sex-specific results in previous DFBC-studies, we subsequently performed the analyses separately for men and women. To evaluate potential participation bias we explored differences in birth weight among those not eligible, those eligible participating and those eligible not participating in the current study.

Results

In total, 134 (22.7%; excluding 4 missing) participants reported to have some problems with their cognition. Only two (0.3%) participants reported severe problems, therefore, we merged some/severe problems in further analyses. Thirty-eight individuals (6.4%; excluding 1 missing) reported to have consulted a healthcare practitioner for cognitive problems in the past 12 months (12 individuals did not report these problems in the first question). Age, sex, birth characteristics, education and SES were not associated with reporting cognitive problems or consulting a healthcare practitioner for these problems (Additional file 1). Individuals who were living alone or scored more than 7 points on either the anxiety or depression scale (either at a mean age of 58, 63 or 72) reported significantly more cognitive problems than individuals who were not living alone or scored 7 or less points on the anxiety or depression scale. The same applied to men and women with hypertension, hypercholesterolemia, vascular problems or diabetes at a mean age of 58 or 63, however, only some of these associations were statistically significant (Additional file 1). Most baseline characteristics were similar, however, some pregnancy, birth and adult characteristics differed among the exposure and control groups (Table 1).

Table 1 General, maternal, pregnancy, birth and adult characteristics by timing of prenatal exposure to the Dutch famine

Overall, the prevalence of reporting cognitive problems did not differ between those unexposed and those prenatally exposed to famine (Table 2). However, those exposed in early gestation more often reported having consulted a healthcare practitioner for cognitive problems in the past 12 months than controls (OR: 3.2 [95% CI 1.3–8.1]). Looking at men and women separately showed that women exposed in late gestation more often reported cognitive problems (OR 2.2 [1.1–4.4]), whereas men exposed in early gestation more often reported cognitive problems (OR 2.3 [0.9–5.5]) than those who had not been exposed to famine prenatally. Men exposed in early gestation four times more often reported having consulted a healthcare practitioner for cognitive problems than men who had not been exposed prenatally (OR 4.4 [1.2–16.0]). Adjusting for covariates did not attenuate the estimates.

Table 2 Association of prenatal exposure to famine with self-reported cognitive problems compared to individuals born before or conceived after the famine

Discussion

Our findings suggest that prenatal undernutrition increases the risk of cognitive problems in later life, which also leads to more often consulting a healthcare practitioner for these problems. This increased risk appears to depend on sex and timing during gestation. Particularly men who had been exposed to famine in early gestation more often reported cognitive problems and consulting a healthcare practitioner for these problems. This is in line with the poorer cognitive function in their late fifties [5]. Interestingly, women exposed to famine in late gestation more often reported cognitive problems.

Agreement with other studies

To our knowledge, no other studies have investigated associations between prenatal undernutrition and self-perceived cognitive problems later in life. Studies regarding the Chinese Great Leap Forward famine of 1959–61 did show that men and women prenatally exposed to undernutrition had more cognitive impairments (measured with cognitive tests) and dementia than controls [10, 11]. However, most Chinese famine studies are hampered by only selecting controls born after the famine, thereby not accounting for age differences (a major risk factor) and potentially overestimating their results. In the DFBC, we account for this bias by also including controls born before the famine. Furthermore, studies have linked other adverse prenatal and early-life circumstances to late-life cognitive problems and dementia [1,2,3,4, 6]. Good economic conditions at the time of birth have for instance been associated with a higher chance of good cognitive functioning at age 60 and older, while a recession was associated with lower cognitive functioning at those ages [12]. Only one study investigated, and found, an association between a prenatal exposure (maternal hypertensive disorder) and subjective cognitive complaints [13].

Interpretation and potential mechanisms

In previous DFBC-studies, men exposed in early gestation were shown to have smaller total brain volume and increased BrainAGE at age 68 [14, 15]. These findings may point at a compromised brain reserve, which may result in an earlier crossing of the threshold below which normal cognitive functioning cannot be sustained [4]. Furthermore, animal studies have indicated that early-life circumstances can modify the rate of accumulation of dementia-related pathologies [2]. Additionally, as hypothesized previously [6], our current observations could be due to vascular effects. Prior DFBC-studies showed increased risks for cardiovascular and metabolic diseases in exposed individuals, as well as overall worse brain perfusion [5, 16]. Building on this, the association we observed is likely not a direct effect alone. We lacked statistical power to formally explore mediators, however, we observed more reporting of cognitive problems in individuals with hypercholesterolemia or hypertension around age 58 and 63 in our cohort. Furthermore, high anxiety and depression scores measured at a mean age of 58, 63 and 72 were strongly associated with self-perceived cognitive problems. Anxiety and depression symptoms seem more prevalent among men and women who had been exposed in early gestation compared to controls, similar to what we have observed before in men exposed in early gestation [17]. Associations with anxiety and depression symptoms reported 10 years earlier would suggest a possible role for these symptoms as mediating factors. Some studies have indicated that earlier depressive episodes may confer increased risk for later pathologic cognitive decline [18]. However, the relationship between depression and anxiety - especially in older adults - and self-perceived cognitive problems is complex [7]. Anxiety and depression may influence the awareness and occurrence of cognitive problems, yet, anxiety and depression in older adults have been associated with objective measures of brain aging and increased risk for dementia [7].

There are differences in the prenatal development of men and women, men are, for instance, more vulnerable to certain adverse prenatal circumstances [19]. In earlier studies in the same cohort, we have observed sex-specific effects similar to the ones described in the current study, the effects of exposure to famine in early gestation were more pronounced in men than in women. These results could be due to the increased vulnerability of men due to their early life growth strategy [20]). Alternatively, excess mortality in women exposed early in gestation, may have led to an underestimation of the effect in women as relatively healthier women remain [21].

Strengths and limitations

Strengths of the current study include that the results are in line with previous findings and seem relatively robust. We made use of a unique cohort with a quasi-experimental set up, which limits the risks of bias and confounding [5, 6].

This study has limitations. The number of participants was small, thus we had limited power in our analyses, especially regarding consulting a healthcare practitioner for cognitive problems and the sex-stratified analyses. A limitation of the current study is the lack of objective information about cognitive status and/or dementia diagnosis in the cohort. Regardless, we do observe significant associations. Furthermore, only 50.0% of the initial cohort was eligible to participate in 2018, and of this group, 49.3% participated in the current study. Birth weights were similar among these groups, however, adult characteristics may differ and selective participation may have caused bias. Individuals with (early stage) dementia may, for instance, be less likely to participate in questionnaires, and when they do, may fail to recognize their cognitive problems [7]. However, as our study sample is relatively healthy and relatively young we expect to mainly see early and less severe symptoms of cognitive decline. As prenatal famine exposure has previously been associated with poorer health outcomes, we expect a relatively greater loss of participants with poorer health in the exposed groups [22]. Both participation bias and possible misrepresentation of dementia diagnoses would most likely lead to an underestimation of the associations, this may especially be true for people who had been exposed in early gestation who seemed most affected by brain aging and health problems [5, 6, 14,15,16]. Furthermore, adding covariates to the models did not substantially attenuate our estimates, which adds to the robustness of our findings, however, the possibility of residual confounding remains.

Regarding the exposure, we cannot be absolutely sure that our associations are solely due to prenatal undernutrition. Maternal stress due to the food scarcity and war conditions may have also played a role [23]. The stress experienced by pregnant women during the famine was likely more extreme than the stress experienced by women before or after the famine [23]. Regarding the outcome, the current study focused on a subjective measure of cognitive aging. Although objective measures of cognitive and brain aging remain the gold standard, self-perceived cognitive problems are of importance. Self-perceived cognitive problems usually occur before any other objective measures of cognitive problems and can be predictive for future objective cognitive decline and the development of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementia [7, 24]. Studies have suggested that self-perceived cognitive problems represent one of the earliest symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease [7, 24]. Furthermore, subjective cognitive problems have been associated with measures of brain aging, like hippocampal atrophy and greater white matter hyperintensity volume [25]. Self-perceived cognitive problems, in our sample, show - to be expected - associations with measures of depression and anxiety, strengthening our confidence in their validity [7]. As described earlier the relationship between self-perceived cognitive problems and anxiety and depression is complex. One study showed associations between self-perceived cognitive problems and more objective cognitive decline and higher risk of MCI and dementia at the end of follow-up, while excluding participants with clinical depression [24]. These findings would indicate that the predictive value of self-perceived cognitive problems is not solely dependent on the presence of depressive symptoms.

Interestingly, 12 individuals reported to have consulted a healthcare practitioner for cognitive problems while they did not report having cognitive problems. Possibly, this discrepancy is due to others worrying about the participants’ cognitive health. Excluding these individuals from the analyses regarding consulting a healthcare practitioner led to slightly higher effect estimates regarding the association between prenatal undernutrition and self-perceived cognitive problems, suggesting we might underestimate our findings by including these individuals.

Conclusion

Our results suggest that prenatal undernutrition increases the risk of self-perceived cognitive problems in men and women, and that this effect depends on sex and the timing during gestation. Possibly, these self-perceived cognitive problems are predictive of further cognitive decline and dementia [7]. Future studies will investigate dementia prevalence in these men and women as they age.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets supporting the conclusion of this article are not available in a public repository.

Data can be made available by the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Change history

Abbreviations

DFBC:

Dutch Famine Birth Cohort

CI:

Confidence Interval

HADS:

Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale

OR:

Odds Ratio

SES:

Socioeconomic Status

TIA:

Transient Ischemic Attack

TOPICS-MDS:

The Older Persons and Informal Caregivers Survey – Minimum DataSet

References

  1. Seifan A, Schelke M, Obeng-Aduasare Y, Isaacson R. Early life epidemiology of Alzheimer’s disease--a critical review. Neuroepidemiology. 2015;45:237–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Lesuis SL, Hoeijmakers L, Korosi A, de Rooij SR, Swaab DF, Kessels HW, et al. Vulnerability and resilience to Alzheimer’s disease: early life conditions modulate neuropathology and determine cognitive reserve. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2018;10:95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Raikkonen K, Kajantie E, Pesonen AK, Heinonen K, Alastalo H, Leskinen JT, et al. Early life origins cognitive decline: findings in elderly men in the Helsinki birth cohort study. PLoS One. 2013;8:e54707.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Borenstein AR, Copenhaver CI, Mortimer JA. Early-life risk factors for Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2006;20:63–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Roseboom TJ, Painter RC, van Abeelen AFM, Veenendaal MVE, de Rooij SR. Hungry in the womb: what are the consequences? Lessons from the Dutch famine. Maturitas. 2011;70:141–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. de Rooij SR, Wouters H, Yonker JE, Painter RC, Roseboom TJ. Prenatal undernutrition and cognitive function in late adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2010;107:16881–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Rabin LA, Smart CM, Amariglio RE. Subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2017;13:369–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. TOPICS-MDS. https://topics-mds.eu/survey-instrument/. Accessed 8 Sep 2020.

  9. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1983;67:361–70.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Kang Y, Zhang Y, Feng Z, Liu M, Li Y, Yang H, et al. Nutritional deficiency in early life facilitates aging-associated cognitive decline. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2017;14:841–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Wang C, An Y, Yu H, Feng L, Liu Q, Lu Y, et al. Association between exposure to the Chinese famine in different stages of early life and decline in cognitive functioning in adulthood. Front Behav Neurosci. 2016;10:146.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Doblhammer G, van den Berg GJ, Fritze T. Economic conditions at the time of birth and cognitive abilities late in life: evidence from ten European countries. PLoS One. 2013;8:e74915.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Tuovinen S, Eriksson JG, Kajantie E, Lahti J, Pesonen AK, Heinonen K, et al. Maternal hypertensive disorders in pregnancy and self-reported cognitive impairment of the offspring 70 years later: the Helsinki Birth Cohort Study. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2013;208:200.e1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. De Rooij SR, Caan MWA, Swaab DF, Nederveen AJ, Majoie CB, Schwab M, et al. Prenatal famine exposure has sex-specific effects on brain size. Brain. 2016;139:2136–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Franke K, Gaser C, Roseboom TJ, Schwab M, de Rooij SR. Premature brain aging in humans exposed to maternal nutrient restriction during early gestation. Neuroimage. 2018;173:460–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. de Rooij SR, Mutsaerts HJMM, Petr J, Asllani I, Caan MWA, Groot P, et al. Late-life brain perfusion after prenatal famine exposure. Neurobiol Aging. 2019;82:1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. De Rooij SR, Painter RC, Phillips DI, Rikknen K, Schene AH, Roseboom TJ. Self-reported depression and anxiety after prenatal famine exposure: mediation by cardio-metabolic pathology? J Dev Orig Health Dis. 2011;2:136–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Butters MA, Young JB, Lopez O, Aizenstein HJ, Mulsant BH, Reynolds CF, et al. Pathways linking late-life depression to persistent cognitive impairment and dementia. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2008;10:345.

  19. Aiken CE, Ozanne SE. Sex differences in developmental programming models. Reproduction. 2013;145:R1–13.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Eriksson JG, Kajantie E, Osmond C, Thornburg K, Barker DJP. Boys live dangerously in the womb. Am J Hum Biol. 2010;22:330–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Van Abeelen AFM, Veenendaal MVE, Painter RC, De Rooij SR, Dijkgraaf MGW, Bossuyt PMM, et al. Survival effects of prenatal famine exposure. Am J Clin Nutr. 2012;95:179–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Bleker LS, De Rooij SR, Painter RC, Ravelli ACJ, Roseboom TJ. Cohort profile: the Dutch famine birth cohort (DFBC) -a prospective birth cohort study in the Netherlands. BMJ Open. 2021;11:e042078.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. De Rooij SR, Bleker LS, Painter RC, Ravelli AC, Roseboom TJ. Lessons learned from 25 years of research into long term consequences of prenatal exposure to the Dutch famine 1944–45: the Dutch famine birth cohort. Int J Environ Health Res. 2021;1-15.

  24. Kielb S, Rogalski E, Weintraub S, Rademaker A. Objective features of subjective cognitive decline in a United States national database. Alzheimers Dement. 2017;13:1337–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Van Rooden S, Van Den Berg-Huysmans AA, Croll PH, Labadie G, Hayes JM, Viviano R, et al. Subjective cognitive decline is associated with greater white matter Hyperintensity volume. J Alzheimers Dis. 2018;66:1283-94.

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank the participants of the Dutch famine birth cohort for their willing cooperation in this study.

Funding

This work was supported by the European Commission Horizon 2020, project DynaHealth (633595) and project LongITools (874739); and a Dutch Research Council (NWO) Aspasia grant (015014039) awarded to S.R. de Rooij. The funding organizations had no role in any part of this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Study concept and design (TR, SR), acquisition of data (SR), analysis (AMW) and interpretation of data (AMW, AB, TR, SR). AMW drafted the manuscript and all authors critically reviewed and revised the manuscript. The author(s) read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Aline Marileen Wiegersma.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The Medical Ethics Review Committee of the Academic Medical Center concluded that a full review and official approval of this study was not required according to Dutch law. All participants provided written informed consent.

All methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

Association of covariates with self-perceived cognitive problems. Description: We explored associations between covariates and self-perceived cognitive problems with logistic regression analyses.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wiegersma, A.M., Boots, A., Roseboom, T.J. et al. Prenatal exposure to the Dutch famine is associated with more self-perceived cognitive problems at 72 years of age. BMC Geriatr 22, 176 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-022-02820-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-022-02820-2

Keywords