Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Socially desirable responding in geriatric outpatients with and without mild cognitive impairment and its association with the assessment of self-reported mental health

A Correction to this article was published on 22 November 2021

This article has been updated

Abstract

Background

Socially desirable responding is a potentially relevant issue in older adults and can be evaluated with the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale (MCSDS). However, the eight-item MCSDS has never been specifically administered to geriatric subjects, and there is a dearth of literature on the relationship between social desirability and cognitive impairment. Also, the connection between social desirability and subjective measures of psychological well-being is a matter of controversy. This study has three main aims. First, to determine the psychometric properties of the eight-item MCSDS in geriatric outpatients without dementia (i.e. with normal cognition (NC) or mild cognitive impairment (MCI)). Second, to investigate the link between social desirability and cognitive functioning. Third, to determine the association between social desirability and the assessment of self-reported mental health.

Methods

Community-dwelling outpatients (aged ≥ 65) were consecutively recruited and neuropsychologically tested to diagnose NC or MCI (n = 299). Social desirability was assessed with the eight-item MCSDS. Depressive and anxiety symptoms were measured with the short Geriatric Depression (GDS-s) and the State-Trait Personality Inventory Trait Anxiety (STPI-TA) scales.

Results

On principal components analysis, the eight-item MCSDS was found to have a multidimensional structure. Of the initial three-component solution, only two subscales had acceptable internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha > 0.6): “Acceptance of responsibility” and “Integrity”. The third subscale (“Kindness towards others”) appeared to gauge two distinct constructs of formal (i.e. politeness) versus substantive (i.e. forgiveness) compassion. On binary logistic regression, only higher income was a significant predictor of formal compassion. Test-retest reliability was substantial to excellent (Gwet’s AC2 ≥ 0.8). There were no meaningful differences in social desirability between the NC and MCI groups. Likewise, negative Spearman’s correlations between social desirability and cognitive Z-scores across the whole sample were weak (rs < |0.3|) and confined to one MCSDS item. Although social desirability was an independent predictor of the STPI-TA score in multiple linear regression, it explained only a marginal amount of incremental variance in anxiety symptoms (less than 2%).

Conclusions

Our results suggest that social desirability need not be a major concern when using questionnaires to assess mental health in geriatric outpatients without dementia.

Peer Review reports

Background

Social desirability is the tendency of subjects to respond to self-report items in a manner that portrays them in an overly favourable light with respect to prevailing social norms and standards [1]. It can therefore affect all self-rated measures, especially those involving socially sensitive topics like psychological well-being [2, 3].

There are several lines of evidence that support the notion that socially desirable responding may be an important issue among older individuals. First, geriatric clinical practice and ageing research rely heavily on self-reports [4]. Second, it is acknowledged that social desirability increases with age (e.g. [5,6,7,8,9]), be it because older people compensate for the negative way ageing is stereotyped in our society by presenting a more positive image of themselves [10] or because they have more traditional values and are more sensitive to socially accepted norms [7]. Third, mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is highly prevalent in geriatric populations [11] and it could influence social desirability in two opposite directions. On the one hand, the stigma associated with the condition [12] could make subjects more prone to strategic self-presentation [10], thereby increasing social desirability. On the other hand, deficits in social cognitive abilities [13, 14] could make subjects less aware of common social norms, thereby reducing social desirability.

The most widely used tool to assess socially desirable responding is the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale (MCSDS) [15] whose shorter versions (e.g. [16]) minimise time and fatigue and can therefore be better suited to the setting of a comprehensive geriatric assessment.

There are a number of gaps in the literature. The eight-item MCSDS used by Ray and coworkers [16] has never been specifically administered to subjects in the geriatric age range (i.e. aged 65 or more). Also, we are aware of only one study that has investigated the relationship between social desirability and measures of cognition. Indeed, Dijkstra et al. [7] report a negative correlation between the two, but their results may be very specific to the scale employed. In fact, while most short forms of the MCSDS are balanced in terms of positively- and negatively-keyed items [17], they have used the 12-item Eysenck Lie Scale which is composed of mainly (75%) negatively-keyed items (i.e. items for which disagreement indicates socially desirable responding). This leads the authors to hypothesise that poorer cognitive performance produces greater socially desirable responding because difficulties in retrieving information from memory make the respondents more likely to give a “no” answer. Lastly, the extent to which social desirability can impact self-reported psychological data, and hence the assessment of emotional well-being, is still a matter of debate, with some studies reporting an effect [8, 18,19,20] and others reporting none [21,22,23,24,25,26].

Aims

The current study has three aims:

  1. a)

    To evaluate the psychometric properties (internal consistency, unidimensionality and test-retest reliability) of a short, eight-item, form of the MCSDS [16] when applied to geriatric outpatients without dementia (i.e. with normal cognition (NC) or MCI).

  2. b)

    To investigate the relationship between social desirability and cognitive functioning.

  3. c)

    To determine the association between social desirability and self-reported symptoms of depression and anxiety.

Methods

Participants

In this cross-sectional study we enrolled 359 community-dwelling older subjects (aged ≥ 65), without a known diagnosis of dementia, who consecutively attended a first geriatric visit at the Geriatric Outpatient Unit of our university hospital in Milan, Italy, from January 2018 to January 2019. After a general assessment, participants were invited to undergo an on-site neuropsychological evaluation. 299 subjects diagnosed with NC (n = 117) or MCI (n = 182) were administered the three scales (MCSDS and the scales for depressive and anxiety symptoms, see later) one month after the diagnosis. The scales were administered during a one-to-one interview with a geriatrician. In accordance with previous research [20,21,22,23], statements from the questionnaires were read to the respondent in order to facilitate understanding and minimise fatigue. The order of the scales was counterbalanced across participants to control for order effects.

To determine test-test reliability a random sample of 50 subjects were administered the MCSDS a second time, one month after the first administration. The size of this subsample was based on the recommendation that at least 30 subjects are required for reliability studies [27]. The time interval was chosen because it is the one originally used by Crowne and Marlowe [15] and it appears to strike a reasonable balance between the need to minimise recollection bias while ensuring clinical stability.

General assessment

Information was collected on the sociodemographic characteristics of the participants: age, sex, education and income. Since income is a sensitive variable, participants were only asked to disclose if their monthly income was above or below the 1500 euro threshold, which is the reported mean for Italian pensioners [28].

The Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) [29], corrected for age and education, was used to provide a crude measure of global cognition. Functional status was evaluated with the scales for the Basic Activities of Daily Living (BADL) [30] and the Instrumental Activities of Daily Living (IADL) [31]. Comorbidity was quantified by the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale comorbidity (CIRS-m) score [32].

Neuropsychological assessment

The neuropsychological assessment was carried out by means of a comprehensive battery of tests investigating different cognitive domains: attention [33, 34], memory [35], executive functions [34, 36,37,38,39,40], language [34, 41], visuospatial skills [34, 42] and ideomotor praxis [43]. The neuropsychological tests are reported in Table 1. We computed a global cognitive Z-score for use as a fine-grained measure of cognitive function. The raw score from each neuropsychological test was transformed to a Z-score, based on the mean and standard deviation of the normative score distribution, and scores that quantified response time or number of errors were multiplied by − 1 so that lower Z-scores always indicated poorer performance. The Z-scores were then averaged across all tests to generate a composite. Since the medial temporal lobe (involved in memory) and the prefrontal cortex (involved in attention and executive functioning) have been shown to be neural substrates of social cognition [14], we also generated separate Z-scores for memory and attention/executive functioning.

Table 1 Neuropsychological tests and cognitive domains assessed

MCI was diagnosed according to current consensus criteria of objective cognitive impairment on neuropsychological testing, essentially preserved daily functioning (i.e. intact BADL with no or minimal impairment of IADL) and no dementia [44]. Objective cognitive impairment was defined by at least one neuropsychological test having a score below the 10th percentile of the normative score distribution [45, 46].

Assessment of social desirability

Social desirability was assessed with a short version of the MCSDS [16]. It consists of eight statements in question form describing socially desirable but uncommon behaviours (e.g. being always polite) and socially undesirable but common behaviours (e.g. being sometimes unforgiving). There are four positively-keyed and four negatively-keyed statements. A “yes” answer is scored 1 for negatively-keyed statements and 3 for positively-keyed statements. A “no” answer is reverse scored. A “don’t know” or missing answer is scored 2. Hence, scores range from 8 to 24, with higher scores indicating greater social desirability. The eight-item MCSDS can be found in Additional file 1.

Assessment of depressive and anxiety symptoms

Depressive symptoms were assessed with the short, 15-item, form of the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS-s) [47]. Answers are in a yes/no format and scores range from 0 to 15, with higher scores indicating greater depressive symptoms.

Anxiety symptoms were assessed with the Trait Anxiety (TA) scale from Spielberger’s State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI) (STPI-TA) [48]. Answers are given on a 4-point Likert scale for the frequency of symptoms and scores range from 10 to 40, with higher scores indicating greater anxiety symptoms.

Both scales have been extensively validated in geriatric populations (e.g. [49, 50]), also including subjects with MCI (e.g. [51,52,53,54]).

Statistical analysis

The statistical analysis was performed by means of the statistical packages SPSS version 26.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL) and R version 3.5.2 (The R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria) for Windows. Parametric and non-parametric statistics were chosen as appropriate. Normality was assessed by visual inspection of QQ plots; linearity and homoscedasticity were assessed by visual inspection of residual versus predictor plots; lack of multicollinearity was assessed by the Variance Inflation Factor (VIF < 5); independence of errors was assessed by the Durbin-Watson test (values 1.97–2.06). The NC and MCI groups were compared by means of Student’s t-test or Mann-Whitney’s U-test for continuous variables and by means of the Chi-squared test for categorical variables. The internal consistency of the MCSDS was quantified by Cronbach’s alpha and by the average inter-item correlation. The latter was specifically evaluated since it is considered to be a better marker of internal consistency [55, 56]. In fact, it is recognised that Cronbach’s alpha depends on both the average inter-item correlation and the number of items in the scale [57], so that it can be high for lengthy scales with weak inter-item correlations and relatively low for short scales with stronger inter-item correlations. The factorial structure of the MCSDS was explored by conducting a principal components analysis (PCA) with an oblique rotation (direct oblimin), which is the most conservative since it allows for correlations between factors. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy (> 0.5) and Bartlett’s test of sphericity (< 0.05) indicated that the data were suitable for PCA. Components were extracted according to Kaiser’s criterion (eigenvalue > 1) [58]. Factor loadings were considered significant if they had an absolute value ≥ 0.60 [59, 60]. Test-retest reliability was evaluated with Gwet’s agreement coefficient (AC) which is a chance-corrected agreement statistic that, unlike Cohen’s kappa, does not underestimate reliability when there is a high prevalence of one response category [61, 62]. In particular, linear-weighted Gwet’s AC2 for interval data was used [61,62,63,64]. Although the correlation coefficient is a suboptimal measure of test-retest reliability because it does not assess the extent of agreement between variables and as such is vulnerable to systematic bias [65, 66], the test-retest reliability of the total MCSDS score was also calculated with Spearman’s correlation for comparison with other studies [67]. The relationship between the MCSDS and cognitive function scores was evaluated with Spearman’s simple and partial correlations. The association between social desirability and psychological symptoms was investigated by means of Spearman’s simple correlations and multiple linear regression. Sociodemographic and clinical characteristics were considered potential confounders and adjusted for in correlation and regression analyses. A p value ≤ 0.05 was taken to be statistically significant. Because of the exploratory nature of the study we did not correct for multiple testing [68].

A power determination analysis was conducted for the current sample size with G*power software [69] for Mann-Whitney’s U-test and multiple linear regression, and with the formula by Bonett and Wright for Spearman’s correlations [70]. In order to be conservative, given the novelty of the study, we assumed small expected effects sizes (r = 0.2, f2 = 0.04). The achieved sample size of almost 300 participants had a 93% power, with a 5% alpha level (two-tailed), for both Spearman’s correlations and multiple linear regression. Also, considering a minimal clinically significant difference of 1.7 points on the MCSDS [71], based on the standard deviation of previous data from Ray and Lovejoy [72] on the administration of the eight-item MCSDS to subjects in the fifth age decade, Mann-Whitney’s U-test had a 98% power, with a 5% alpha level (two-tailed), to detect a difference between the two groups.

With regard to the test-retest sample size, a power determination was performed with G*power, based on the reported test-retest reliability coefficients for different versions of the MCSDS (r = 0.4 to 0.9) [67]. Considering the lowest value as the most conservative, the n = 50 sample size provided 90% power with a 5% alpha level (one-tailed).

Results

Sample characteristics

Table 2 summarises the main sociodemographic and clinical characteristics of the participants. As expected, in the MCI group education and cognitive and IADL scores were lower, while age was greater. The total MCSDS score had a mean (standard deviation) of 20.0 (2.6) in the whole sample, 19.6 (2.6) in the NC group and 20.2 (2.5) in the MCI group.

Table 2 Main sociodemographic and clinical characteristics of the sample

Psychometric properties of the eight-item MCSDS

The psychometric properties of the eight-item MCSDS were primarily evaluated across the whole sample because both subjects with NC and MCI (i.e. without dementia) would be expected to be able to understand and answer the questionnaire and pooling data maximises statistical power for reliability and PC analyses [73, 74].

All participants completed the questionnaire and there were no missing answers. There were only 22 “don’t know” answers overall and only 15 participants (i.e. 5% of the sample) responded to the questionnaire by giving at least one “don’t know” answer.

The internal consistency of the scale was found to be poor: Cronbach’s alpha for the overall sample was 0.42 (for the NC group = 0.35, for the MCI group = 0.46, no significant difference in Cronbach’s alphas between groups according to Feldt’s test [75]: p = 0.289). The mean inter-item correlation was also well below the recommended 0.15–0.50 range [55, 56] (i.e. 0.09), meaning that the brevity of the scale was not a reason for the low Cronbach’s alpha.

The PCA extracted three components, as shown in Table 3. Items 3 and 4 loaded highly on component 1, which involves admitting to one’s mistakes and can be interpreted as “Acceptance of responsibility”. Items 1 and 2 loaded highly on component 2, which relates to abidance to moral values and can be labelled “Integrity”. Items 5 and 7 loaded highly on component 3, which reflects compassionate interpersonal behaviour and can be designated as “Kindness towards others”. Orthogonal (varimax) rotation yielded the same factorial structure, in accordance with the observed weak correlations between factors.

Table 3 PCA with rotated factor loadingsa for the three-component factor solution

The three-component structure of the scale was replicated in both NC and MCI subjects (see Additional file 2).

Cronbach’s alphas were acceptable (i.e. > 0.6) [60, 76] and mean inter-item correlations were high (i.e. > 0.5) for the first two components. However, for the third component Cronbach’s alpha was low and the mean inter-item correlation was just acceptable.

On further analysis, this could, at least in part, be ascribed to the fact there was a consistent proportion of participants (31%) who responded to the two constituent questions in an incongruent manner (Fig. 1). In fact, several subjects (16%) answered item 5 in a socially desirable way and item 7 in a socially undesirable way, i.e. they reported never trying to get even rather than forgive and forget, but admitted to sometimes not being courteous to people. Viceversa, several others (15%) answered item 5 in a socially undesirable way and item 7 in a socially desirable way, i.e. they acknowledged trying sometimes to get even rather than forgive and forget, but professed to be always courteous, even to disagreeable people.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Pattern of responses to the “Kindness towards others” subscale. Congruent responding corresponds to the quadrants labelled Social desirability and Social undesirability. Incongruent responding corresponds to the quadrants labelled Substantive compassion and Formal compassion (in italics)

It thus appeared that the underlying construct of “Kindness towards others” could be separated into two distinct concepts: one of a more substantive nature (i.e. being forgiving but not polite), and one of a more formal nature (i.e. being polite but not forgiving). We performed a binary logistic regression with the nature of the concept (formal vs substantive) as the dependent variable, and sociodemographic and clinical characteristics (age, sex, education, income, global cognition Z-score; GDS-s, STPI-TA and CIRS-m scores) as independent variables which were entered simultaneously in the model. The linearity (Box-Tidwell transformation) and no multicollinearity (VIF < 5) assumptions were satisfied. The only independent predictor of the pattern of incongruent responding was income, i.e. participants with higher income were more likely to report formal rather than substantive kindness towards others (Odds Ratio 3.2, 95% Confidence Interval 1.1–9.5, p = 0.038, see Additional file 3).

Since the MCSDS was found to have a multidimensional structure, i.e. it was composed of items tapping different underlying constructs, we believed it would be more conceptually appropriate to conduct the subsequent statistical analyses (see following sections) on scores from the two valid subscales (“Acceptance of responsibility” and “Integrity”) and from the other four individual items. This choice is in line with the general acceptance in the psychometric literature [77,78,79,80] that the total score of a scale lacking unidimensionality cannot be meaningfully interpreted because it represents a mixture of several facets. However, for the sake of completeness, we performed the same analyses on the total MCSDS score and found no significant differences in the pattern of results (data not shown).

As far as test-retest reliability was concerned, Gwet’s AC2 ranged from 0.79 to 0.93 (see Additional file 4) and Spearman’s correlation coefficient for the total MCSDS score was 0.76.

Relationship between social desirability and cognitive functioning

The relationship between social desirability and cognitive functioning was explored in two ways: cognitive functioning was first treated as a dichotomous variable (NC vs MCI categorisation) and then as a continuous variable (cognitive Z-scores). The second approach has two main strengths: it prevents loss of information, increasing the sensitivity of the statistical analysis [81], and it acknowledges the continuum nature of cognitive ageing [82]. Thus, the two groups were first compared on the MCSDS scores and then the correlations (unadjusted and adjusted) between the MCSDS and cognitive Z-scores were evaluated across the whole sample. The results are shown in Tables 4 and 5.

Table 4 MCSDS scores in the two cognitive groups
Table 5 Spearman’s correlations between MCSDS and cognitive function scores

The higher score of MCI subjects on item 7 of the MCSDS was statistically significant, but not clinically relevant. Item 7 of the MCSDS exhibited significant, albeit weak, negative correlations with all cognitive Z scores on bivariate testing (rs < |0.3|). These correlations were retained on multivariate testing, after partialling out the effects of sociodemographic and clinical variables, with the Z score for memory achieving borderline statistical significance.

When the correlation analyses were performed separately in the NC and MCI subjects no significant differences were found between groups by Fisher’s r to z transformation test (see Additional file 5).

Relationship between social desirability and psychological symptoms

The relationship between social desirability and symptoms of depression and anxiety was investigated in a three-step process.

First, we calculated Spearman’ s bivariate correlations between each of the GDS-s and STPI-TA scores and the six MCSDS scores across the whole sample. The results are shown in Table 6. The GDS-s scale was found to have significant, albeit weak (rs < |0.3|), negative correlations with the “Acceptance of responsibility” subscale and items 5 and 6 of the MCSDS. The STPI-TA scale was found to have significant/borderline significant, albeit weak (rs < |0.3|), negative correlations with the “Acceptance of responsibility” subscale and item 6 of the MCSDS. The correlation between the GDS-s and STPI-TA scales was, instead, moderate and positive (r s = 0.53, p < 0.001). When the correlation analyses were performed separately in the NC and MCI subjects no significant differences were found between groups by Fisher’s r to z transformation test (see Additional file 6).

Table 6 Simple Spearman’s correlations between MCSDS and GDS-s and STPI-TA scores

We then employed multiple linear regression over the whole sample in order to investigate whether social desirability was a predictor of psychological well-being after controlling for sociodemographic and clinical characteristics. We fitted separate models for each of the two mental health scales and for each of the six MCSDS scores (full model). Thus, the dependent variable was either the GDS-s score (first six models) or the STPI-TA score (last six models) and the independent variables were the individual MCSDS score (the variable of interest) as well as the potential confounders: age, sex, education, income, global cognitive Z score, comorbidity, GDS-score (last six models) and STPI-TA score (first six models).

Lastly, for each regression we also computed a reduced model, without the MCSDS score, to quantify the amount of additional variance explained by the inclusion of the MCSDS score in the model, via the change in R squared statistic. The lack of significant group by MCSDS score interactions rendered it appropriate to collapse NC and MCI subjects into a single group. The results are summarised in Table 7. Regression analyses in which cognition was modelled as a dichotomous variable (NC vs MCI group) rather than a continuous one produced the same results (see Additional file 7).

Table 7 MCSDS scores as predictors of depressive and anxiety symptoms

The “Acceptance of responsibility” subscale and items 6 and 7 of the MCSDS were significant independent predictors of the STPI-TA score. However, socially desirable responding accounted only for a very small proportion (less than 2%) of additional variance in the STPI-TA score after controlling for sociodemographic and clinical characteristics.

Discussion

In the current study we investigated the psychometric properties of the eight-item MCSDS in geriatric outpatients without dementia as well as the relationship of social desirability with both cognitive functioning and self-reported psychological symptoms. The high prevalence of MCI in the study (61%) is worthy of specific comment. The percentage of older adults classified as MCI has been consistently shown to vary widely across studies due to several methodological factors such as recruitment source, type and number of tests used to assess cognition, and operationalisation of the MCI criteria [83]. In our case, the high prevalence of MCI in the sample could have two possible explanations. First, the diagnosis of MCI relied on an extensive neuropsychological battery, included all MCI subtypes and was based on somewhat lenient criteria (10th percentile cut-off in at least one cognitive test, no requirement for subjective cognitive impairment or intact IADL). Second, the study was conducted in an outpatient clinic setting. The latter is presumably the most likely explanation, since the prevalence of MCI, across different operational definitions, ranges from 3 to 42% in community-based studies [84] and from 40 to 84% in specialty outpatient clinics [85, 86].

Psychometric properties of the eight-item MCSDS

The eight-item MCSDS displayed poor internal consistency, in terms of Cronbach’s alpha and mean inter-item correlation, because it lacked unidimensionality, i.e. it did not measure a single construct [87]. Indeed, on PCA it was found to have a multidimensional structure. This in line with reports from other authors investigating both short [17, 88] and full [89, 90] forms of the MCSDS.

In particular, three components were identified: “Acceptance of responsibility”, “Integrity” and “Kindness towards others”. The first component involved admitting to one’s mistakes. The second component reflected abidance to moral values. The third component could be conceptualised as measuring two different constructs: one of a more substantive nature, relating to empathy (i.e. forgiveness but not politeness), and one of a more formal nature, linked to social etiquette (i.e. politeness but not forgiveness).

Interestingly, higher income was an independent predictor of formal rather than substantive “Kindness towards others”. Since income is an indicator of socioeconomic status, this finding fits in nicely with the literature. In fact, there is evidence that people from different social strata endorse different sets of values. Individuals who are higher in social class are more likely to attach importance to good manners [91]. Also, in their moral judgments, they have been shown to prioritise the domain of respect rather than that of no harm to others, while the reverse has been found to be true for individuals who are lower in social standing [92].

Although education is also a proxy for socioeconomic status, it was not found to have the same effect as income. This is probably so because of the geriatric context of the study. In fact, education is mainly a measure of early-life (received) socioeconomic status, while income is an accurate index of late-life (actual) socioeconomic status [93].

Lastly, the test-retest reliability of the MCSDS at one month was found to be substantial to excellent (Gwet’s AC2 ≥ 0.8) [94]. Spearman’s correlation coefficient for the total MCSDS score was excellent (rs = 0.8) [65] and within the higher end of the 0.4 to 0.9 range reported by other studies [67].

Relationship between social desirability and cognitive functioning

There was a statistically significant but not clinically relevant difference in social desirability between the NC and MCI groups, with the MCI subjects scoring slightly higher on item 7 of the MCSDS (“I am always courteous, even to disagreeable people”). Along the same lines, item 7 of the MCSDS exhibited significant negative, albeit weak (rs < |0.3|), Spearman’s correlations with the Z scores for global cognition, memory and attention/executive functioning, even after controlling for potential confounders. Thus, the observed marginal association between social desirability and cognitive functioning was confined to item 7 of the MCSDS, which pertains to politeness. This result seems to resonate with a handful of case studies and case series in the area of sociolinguistic research. They have noted that, in the conversation of people with dementia, social politeness strategies are retained [95,96,97] or indeed enhanced [98, 99], supposedly to mask cognitive symptoms that would damage their social persona [96, 98, 99].

However, given their small effect size, our findings provide no consistent support to the hypothesis that cognitive impairment increases socially desirable responding. It is possible that our sample of geriatric outpatients experienced little stigmatisation due to the MCI label since it has been reported that social discrimination positively correlates with the severity of the cognitive disorder [100] and that in “traditional” countries like Italy less stigma is attached to cognitive impairment than elsewhere in Europe [12].

Also, there was no evidence in favour of the alternative hypothesis that cognitive impairment could decrease socially desirable responding because of deficits in social cognition. Although impairment in social cognition has recently gained attention in MCI [13, 14], studies have primarily relied on theory of the mind (ToM) tasks. It is recognised that ToM tasks gauge only a specific aspect of social cognition (i.e. understanding the mental states of others) [101]. It is also accepted that such laboratory-based measures may overstate the difficulties encountered by older subjects in real-life social interactions, in which a meaningful context may bring about efficient compensation [102]. Thus, it could be speculated that more naturalistic, everyday social skills, including the ability to edit responses in terms of their social desirability implications, could be preserved in MCI.

Relationship between social desirability and psychological symptoms

On correlation analysis there were some significant negative correlations between social desirability and symptoms of depression and anxiety. Although such correlations were weak (rs < |0.3|) and scattered across items of the MCSDS, their direction is in accordance with a large body of literature demonstrating that greater social desirability is associated with higher scores on measures of psychological well-being (e.g. [18, 20, 21, 103]). Likewise, in line with previous research, the correlations between the MCSDS and the well-being scales were weaker than those between the two well-being scales [104].

When multiple linear regression was used to quantify the effect of socially desirable responding on self-rated mental health, after controlling for a number of confounders, we found that social desirability had a statistically significant association with anxiety but not depressive symptoms. Since it would be reasonable to expect the relationship between social desirability and psychological well-being to be more manifest for mental health symptoms that are more prone to stigma, it could be conjectured that anxiety carries a greater stigma burden than depression. As a matter of fact, older adults have been shown to hold more stigmatising attitudes towards their peers with anxiety, whom they perceive as responsible for their condition, than with depression [105]. This is likely to arise from the common misconception - by the public [105, 106], health professionals [107] and depression sufferers themselves [108] – that depression is a normal part of ageing. In any case, such result appears to have little practical relevance, given that socially desirable responding uniquely explained only a small amount of additional variance in anxiety symptoms (i.e. less than 2%) above and beyond sociodemographic and clinical characteristics.

The inconsequential relationship between the MCSDS and the well-being scales documented by the current study is consonant with reports from other authors [21, 22, 24, 25, 104] and contributes to the ongoing debate in the literature on whether socially desirable responding can influence self-reported measures of psychological well-being. In our sample of geriatric outpatients, social desirability had a minimal association with subjective measures of psychological symptoms.

Strengths and limitations

The main strengths of the study include its novelty, the use of a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment to characterise individuals as NC or MCI, and the fairly large sample size.

Some limitations must also be acknowledged. First, the cross-sectional design of the study does not allow causal inference. Second, even if the statistical analyses were controlled for a number of potential confounders, the risk of residual confounding cannot be excluded. Third, the mode of administration of the questionnaire (by a one-to-one interview rather than by self-completion) involved a social interaction and is likely to have increased socially desirable responding [3, 109]. Although several studies have reported no differences between interviewer- and self-administered questionnaire modes in the type of response to sensitive issues [109], further research is warranted to determine whether our results would hold true if the questionnaires were filled in by the participants themselves. Nonetheless, a few remarks should be made. In geriatric practice, self-report questionnaires, including those on mental health like the GDS-s (e.g. [110]) and the STPI-TA (e.g. [50]) are often read out to the respondents because this method carries advantages in older adults: it is suitable for subjects with physical impairments (e.g. visual or motor) as well as low literacy, it is less cognitively demanding in the presence of age-related cognitive decline, and it enhances item response rates since the interviewer can maintain motivation, provide clarification and probe for responses [109]. Indeed, survey research has shown that older adults prefer interviewer-administered modes over self-administered modes [111, 112]. Within this context, for the sake of comparability, the MCSDS would also have to be delivered by an interviewer, and this may be the reason why most research on social desirability in older age [20,21,22,23] has employed a similar strategy. Fourth, we used a mainly binary, eight-item MCSDS to assess social desirability and the sample was somewhat homogeneous in its tendency to score towards the higher end of the range. It is possible that a short MCSDS with a Likert-format (e.g. [113]) could have encouraged more diverse responding, improving internal consistency [114] and providing a more nuanced insight into the magnitude of social desirability. Still, research on older subjects suggests that increasing response options can lead to confusion without increasing response variability [115]. Fifth, we supposed that social desirability in MCI could be either increased or decreased, being potentially affected by stigma and loss of social skills respectively. Since the study was not designed to investigate specific underpinnings of social desirability in cognitive impairment, we did not assess perceived stigma or social cognitive abilities. However, it should be noted that scales that measure stigma are per se subject to the bias of socially desirable responding [116] and this could deeply confound any association between perceived stigma and social desirability (e.g. if subjects with cognitive impairment display greater social desirability because they experience greater stigma they might also be more prone to deny stigma). Moreover, although we did not use specific tests of social cognition, we investigated correlations between MCSDS items and Z-scores for memory and attention/executive functioning. Sixth, we did not correct for multiple testing, so inflation of type I error was not controlled for. Positive findings will therefore need to be confirmed by further studies. Finally, we discussed socially desirable responding as an intentional misrepresentation of the self. This is in keeping with the notion, prevalent in health research, that social desirability is a common source of bias in studies involving self-report measures [117], and with most of the literature on social desirability in older age [20,21,22,23]. Yet, we recognise that the stylistic (i.e. response bias) versus substantive (i.e. stable personality trait) nature of social desirability, and its implications for personality assessment, have long been (e.g. [118]) and still are (e.g. [1, 119]) a matter of debate. The controversy is primarily due to the paucity of studies including an external, objective criterion as benchmark (e.g. ratings from informants) and their conflicting results (e.g. [120,121,122]). Our study did not aim to address this issue since the topic of social desirability is salient to geriatric practice regardless of interpretation [8].

Conclusions

This study is the first to use the eight-item MCSDS in a sample of geriatric outpatients with and without MCI. The scale was found to have a multidimensional structure, including three main subscales: “Acceptance of responsibility”, “Integrity” and “Kindness towards others”. Internal consistency was acceptable for the first two subscales, but not for the third one. In fact, the “Kindness towards others” construct appeared to comprise two distinct concepts of compassionate behaviour - formal (i.e. politeness) and substantive (i.e. forgiveness) - with higher income being the only predictor of formal compassion. Test-retest reliability was substantial to excellent. There was no consistent evidence for an association between cognitive deficits and socially desirable responding. Also, social desirability had a marginal relationship with self-rated depressive and anxiety symptoms.

Our results suggest that social desirability need not be a major concern when using questionnaires to assess mental health in geriatric outpatients without dementia.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Change history

Abbreviations

AC:

Agreement Coefficient

BADL:

Basic Activities of Daily Living

CIRS-m:

Cumulative Illness Rating Scale comorbidity

GDS-s:

short Geriatric Depression Scale

IADL:

Instrumental Activities of Daily Living

MCSDS:

Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale

MCI:

Mild Cognitive Impairment

MMSE:

Mini Mental State Examination

NC:

Normal Cognition

PCA:

Principal Components Analysis

STPI-TA:

State Trait Personality Inventory Trait Anxiety subscale

ToM:

Theory of the Mind

References

  1. Holden RR, Passey J. Social desirability. In: Leary MR, Hoyle RH, editors. Handbook of individual differences in social beahavior. New York: the Guildford Press; 2009. p. 441–54.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Grimm P. Social desirability bias. In Sheth JN, Malhotra NK, editors. Wiley international encyclopedia of marketing. UK: Chichester: John Wiley & Sons Ltd; 2010. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444316568.wiem02057. Accessed 20 Aug 2020.

  3. Krumpal I. Determinants of social desirability bias in sensitive surveys: a literature review. Qual Quant. 2013;47:2025–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Knäuper B, Carrière K, Chamandy M, Xu Z, Schwarz N, Rosen NO. How aging affects self-reports. Eur J Ageing. 2016;13:185–93.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Evans RG. Clinical relevance of the Marlowe-Crowne scale: a review and recommendations. J Pers Assess. 1982;46:415–25.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Ray JJ. Lie scales and the elderly. Personal Individ Differ. 1988;9:417–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Dijkstra W, Smit JH, Comijs HC. Using social desirability scales in research among the elderly. Qual Quant. 2001;35:107–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Soubelet A, Salthouse TA. Influence of social desirability on age differences in self-reports of mood and personality. J Pers. 2011;79:741–62.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Fastame MC, Penna MP. Does social desirability confound the assessment of self-reported measures of well-being and metacognitive efficiency in young and older adults? Clin Gerontol. 2012;35:239–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Martin KA, Leary MR, Rejeski WJ. Self-presentational concerns in older adults: implications for health and well-being. Basic Appl Soc Psychol. 2000;22:169–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Limongi F, Siviero P, Noale M, Gesmundo A, Crepaldi G. Maggi S; dementia registry study group. Prevalence and conversion to dementia of mild cognitive impairment in an elderly Italian population. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2017;29:361–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Lion KM, Szcześniak D, Bulińska K, Evans SB, Evans SC, Saibene FL, et al. Do people with dementia and mild cognitive impairments experience stigma? A cross-cultural investigation between Italy, Poland and the UK. Aging Ment Health. 2019;21:1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Bora E, Yener GG. Meta-analysis of social cognition in mild cognitive impairment. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2017;30:206–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Yi Z, Zhao P, Zhang H, Shi Y, Shi H, Zhong J, et al. Theory of mind in Alzheimer's disease and amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a meta-analysis. Neurol Sci. 2020;41:1027–39.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Crowne DP, Marlowe D. A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. J Consult Psychol. 1960;24:349–54.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Ray JJ. The reliability of short social desirability scales. J Soc Psychol. 1984;123:133–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Ballard R. Short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne social desirability scale. Psychol Rep. 1992;71:1155–60.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Carstensen LL, Cone JD. Social desirability and the measurement of psychological well-being in elderly persons. J Gerontol. 1983;38:713–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Vigil-Colet A, Morales-Vives F, Lorenzo-Seva U. How social desirability and acquiescence affect the age-personality relationship. Psicothema. 2013;25:342–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Fastame MC, Hitchcott PK, Penna MP. Does social desirability influence psychological well-being: perceived physical health and religiosity of Italian elders? A developmental approach. Aging Ment Health. 2017;21:348–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Fastame MC, Hitchcott PK, Penna MP. Do self-referent metacognition and residential context predict depressive symptoms across late-life span? A developmental study in an Italian sample. Aging Ment Health. 2015;19:698–704.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Fastame MC, Penna MP, Hitchcott PK. Life satisfaction and social desirability across the late life span: what relationship? Qual Life Res. 2015;24:241–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Fastame MC, Hitchcott PK, Penna MP, Murino G. Does institutionalization influence perceived metamemory, psychological well-being, and working memory efficiency in Italian elders? A preliminary study. J Clinic Gerontol Geriatr. 2016;7:6–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Lewinsohn PM, Seeley JR, Roberts RE, Allen NB. Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) as a screening instrument for depression among community-residing older adults. Psychol Aging. 1997;12:277–87.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Nuevo R, Montorio I, Márquez-González M, Cabrera I, Izal M, Pérez-Rojo G. Diferencias asociadas a la edad en el efecto de la deseabilidad social en el autoinforme del estado emocional. [age-related differences in the effect of social desirability on self-reported emotional state]. Rev Esp Geriatr Gerontol. 2009;44:85–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Phillips LH, Henry JD, Hosie JA, Milne AB. Age, anger regulation and well-being. Aging Ment Health. 2006;10:250–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. McHugh ML. Interrater reliability: the kappa statistic. Biochem Med (Zagreb). 2012;22:276–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Istituto Nazionale di Statistica (ISTAT). Pensioners' living conditions. (2019). https://www.istat.it/en/archivio/227115. Accessed 20 Aug 2020.

  29. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–98.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Katz S, Downs TD, Cash HR, Grotz RC. Progress in development of the index of ADL. Gerontologist. 1970;10:20–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Lawton MP, Brody EM. Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist. 1969;9:179–86.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Parmelee PA, Thuras PD, Kat IR, Lawton MP. Validation of the cumulative illness rating scale in a geriatric residential population. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1995;43:130–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Vallar G, Rusconi ML, Fontana S, Musicco M. Three clinical tests for the assessment of visuospatial exploration. Norms from 212 normal subjects. Archivio di Psicologia. Neurologia e Psichiatria. 1994;54:827–41.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Spinnler H, Tognoni G. Italian standardization and classification of neuropsychological tests. The Italian group on the neuropsychological study of aging. J Neurol Sci. 1987;8:1–120.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Carlesimo GA, Buccione I, Fadda L, Graceffa A, Mauri M, Lorusso S, et al. Normative data of two memory tasks: short-story recall and Rey's figure. Nuova Riv Neurol. 2002;12:1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Orsini A, Grosso D, Capitani E, Laiacona M, Papagno C, Vallar G. Verbal and spatial immediate memory span: normative data from 1355 adults and 1112 children. Ital J Neurol Sci. 1987;8:539–48.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Monaco M, Costa A, Caltagirone C, Carlesimo GA. Forward and backward span for verbal and visuo-spatial data: standardization and normative data from an Italian adult population. Neurol Sci. 2013;34:749–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Giovagnoli AR, Del Pesce M, Mascheroni S, Simoncelli M, Laiacona M, Capitani E. Trail making test: normative values from 287 normal adult controls. Ital J Neurol Sci. 1996;17:305–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Della Sala S, MacPherson SE, Phillips LH, Sacco L, Spinnler H. How many camels are there in Italy? Cognitive estimates standardised on the Italian population. Neurol Sci. 2003;24:10–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Novelli G, Papagno C, Capitani E, Laiacona M, Cappa SF, Vallar G. Three clinical tests for the assessment of verbal long-term memory function: norms from 320 normal subjects. Arch Psicol Neurol Psichiatr. 1986;47:278–96.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Laiacona M, Barbarotto R, Trivelli C, Capitani E. Dissociazioni semantiche e intercategoriali: descrizione di una batteria standardizzata e dati normativi. Arch Psicol Neurol Psichiatr. 1993;54:209–48.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Caffarra P, Vezzadini G, Dieci F, Zonato F, Venneri A. Rey-Osterrieth complex figure: normative values in an Italian population sample. Neurol Sci. 2002;22:443–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. De Renzi E, Motti F, Nichelli P. Imitating gestures. A quantitative approach to ideomotor apraxia. Arch Neurol. 1980;37:6–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Petersen RC, Caracciolo B, Brayne C, Gauthier S, Jelic V, Fratiglioni L. Mild cognitive impairment: a concept in evolution. J Intern Med. 2014;275:214–28.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Solfrizzi V, Panza F, Colacicco AM, D'Introno A, Capurso C, Torres F, et al. Italian longitudinal study on aging working group. Vascular risk factors, incidence of MCI, and rates of progression to dementia. Neurology. 2004;63:1882–91.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Delano-Wood L, Bondi MW, Sacco J, Abeles N, Jak AJ, Libon DJ, et al. Heterogeneity in mild cognitive impairment: differences in neuropsychological profile and associated white matter lesion pathology. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15:906–14.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Sheikh JI, Yesavage JA. Geriatric depression scale (GDS): recent evidence and development of a shorter version. Clin Gerontol. 1986;5:165–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Spielberger CD, Jacobs G, Crane R, Russell S, Westberry L, Barder L, et al. Preliminary manual for the state-trait personality inventory (STPI). Tampa, FL: University of South Florida Human Resources Institute; 1979.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Wancata J, Alexandrowicz R, Marquart B, Weiss M, Friedrich F. The criterion validity of the geriatric depression scale: a systematic review. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2006;114:398–410.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Bergua V, Meillon C, Potvin O, Ritchie K, Tzourio C, Bouisson J, et al. Short STAI-Y anxiety scales: validation and normative data for elderly subjects. Aging Ment Health. 2016;20:987–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Chiesi F, Primi C, Pigliautile M, Baroni M, Ercolani S, Paolacci L, et al. Does the 15-item geriatric depression scale function differently in old people with different levels of cognitive functioning? J Affect Disord. 2018;227:471–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Coelho S, Guerreiro M, Chester C, Silva D, Maroco J, Coelho M, et al. Time perception in mild cognitive impairment: interval length and subjective passage of time. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2016;22:755–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Ge S, Zhu Z, Wu B, McConnell ES. Technology-based cognitive training and rehabilitation interventions for individuals with mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review. BMC Geriatr. 2018;18:213.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Thoma MV, Forstmeier S, Schmid R, Kellner O, Xepapadakos F, Gasser US, et al. Preliminary evidence for an increased likelihood of a stable trajectory in mild cognitive impairment in individuals with higher motivational abilities. BMC Geriatr. 2018;18:181.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Briggs SR, Cheek JM. The role of factor analysis in the evaluation of personality scales. J Pers. 1986;54:106–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Clark LA, Watson D. Constructing validity: basic issues in objective scale development. Psychol Assess. 1995;7:309–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Cortina JM. What is coefficient alpha? An examination of theory and applications. J Appl Psychol. 1993;78:98–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Kaiser HF. The application of electronic computers to factor analysis. Educ Psychol Meas. 1960;20:141–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Chin WW, Gopal A, Salisbury WD. Advancing the theory of adaptive structuration: the development of a scale to measure faithfulness of appropriation. Inf Syst Res. 1997;8:342–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Hair JE, Back WC, Babin BJ, Rolph EA. Multivariate data analysis. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall; 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Gwet KL. Computing inter-rater reliability and its variance in the presence of high agreement. Br J Math Stat Psychol. 2008;61:29–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Gwet KL. Handbook of inter-rater reliability: the definitive guide to measuring the extent of agreement among raters. Gaithersburg, MD: Advanced Analytics, LLC; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Pasta DJ. Learning when to be discrete: continuous versus categorical predictors. SAS Global Forum; 2009 May; Washington DC, USA. Available from http/support.sas.com/resources/papers/proceedings09/248-2009.pdf.

  64. Norman G. Likert scales, levels of measurement and the "laws" of statistics. Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract. 2010;15:625–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Schober P, Boer C, Schwarte LA. Correlation coefficients: appropriate use and interpretation. Anesth Analg. 2018;126:1763–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Aldridge VK, Dovey TM, Wade A. Assessing test-retest reliability of psychological measures: persistent methodological problems. Eur Psychol. 2017;22:207–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Beretvas SN, Meyers JL, Leite WL. A reliability generalization study of the Marlowe-Crowne social desirability scale. Educ Psychol Meas. 2002;62:570–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Bender R, Lange S. Adjusting for multiple testing--when and how? J Clin Epidemiol. 2001;54:343–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang AG. Statistical power analyses using G*power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods. 2009;41:1149–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Looney SW. Practical issues in sample size determination for correlation coefficient inference. SM J Biometrics Biostat. 2018;3:1027.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Sedaghat AR. Understanding the minimal clinically important difference (MCID) of patient-reported outcome measures. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2019;161:551–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Ray JJ, Lovejoy FH. Age-related social desirability responding among Australian women. J Soc Psychol. 2003;143:669–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Nunnally JC, Bernstein IH. Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  74. Comrey AL, Lee HB. A first course in factor analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1992.

    Google Scholar 

  75. Feldt LS. A test of the hypothesis that Cronbach's alpha or Kuder-Richardson coefficient twenty is the same for two tests. Psychometrika. 1969;34:363–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Wassermann JD, Bracken BA. Handbook of psychology. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  77. Streiner DL. Being inconsistent about consistency: when coefficient alpha does and doesn't matter. J Pers Assess. 2003;80:217–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Slocum-Gori SL, Zumbo BD. Assessing the unidimensionality of psychological scales: using multiple criteria from factor analysis. Soc Indic Res. 2011;102:443–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Ziegler M, Hagemann D. Testing the unidimensionality of items: pitfalls and loopholes. Eur J Psychol Assess. 2015;31:231–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Fried EI, van Borkulo CD, Epskamp S, Schoevers RA, Tuerlinckx F, Borsboom D. Measuring depression over time Or not? Lack of unidimensionality and longitudinal measurement invariance in four common rating scales of depression. Psychol Assess. 2016;28:1354–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Streiner DL. Breaking up is hard to do: the heartbreak of dichotomizing continuous data. Can J Psychiatr. 2002;47:262–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Franceschi C, Garagnani P, Morsiani C, Conte M, Santoro A, Grignolio A, et al. The continuum of aging and age-related diseases: common mechanisms but different rates. Front Med. 2018;5:61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Roberts R, Knopman DS. Classification and epidemiology of MCI. Clin Geriatr Med. 2013;29:753–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Ward A, Arrighi HM, Michels S, Cedarbaum JM. Mild cognitive impairment: disparity of incidence and prevalence estimates. Alzheimers Dement. 2012;8:14–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Pusswald G, Moser D, Gleiss A, Janzek-Hawlat S, Auff E, Dal-Bianco P, et al. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment subtypes in patients attending a memory outpatient clinic--comparison of two modes of mild cognitive impairment classification. Results of the Vienna conversion to dementia study. Alzheimers Dement. 2013;9:366–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Tamura Y, Ishikawa J, Fujiwara Y, Tanaka M, Kanazawa N, Chiba Y, et al. Prevalence of frailty, cognitive impairment, and sarcopenia in outpatients with cardiometabolic disease in a frailty clinic. BMC Geriatr. 2018;18:264.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  87. Tavakol M, Dennic R. Making sense of Cronbach's alpha. Int J Med Educ. 2011;2:53–5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  88. Barger SD. The Marlowe-Crowne affair: short forms, psychometric structure, and social desirability. J Pers Assess. 2002;79:286–305.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Holden RR, Fekken GC. Three common social desirability scales: friends, acquaintances, or strangers? J Res Pers. 1989;23:180–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Leite WL, Beretvas SN. Validation of scores on the Marlowe-Crowne social desirability scale and the balanced inventory of desirable responding. Educ Psychol Meas. 2005;65:140–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  91. Golman R. Good manners: signaling social preferences. Theor Decis. 2016;81:73–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Kraus MW, Piff PK, Mendoza-Denton R, Rheinschmidt ML, Keltner D. Social class, solipsism, and contextualism: how the rich are different from the poor. Psychol Rev. 2012;119:546–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Galobardes B, Shaw M, Lawlor DA, Lynch JW, Davey SG. Indicators of socioeconomic position (part 1). J Epidemiol Community Health. 2006;60:7–12.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  94. Landis JR, Koch GG. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics. 1977;33:159–74.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Temple V, Sabat SR, Kroger R. Intact use of politeness strategies in the discourse of Alzheimer's disease sufferers. Lang Commun. 1999;19:163–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  96. Rhys CS, Schmidtrenfree N. Facework, social politeness and the Alzheimer's patient. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics. 2000;14:533–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  97. Hamilton HE. Narrative as a snapshot: glimpses into the past in Alzheimer’s discourse. Narrat Inq. 2008;8:53–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  98. Guendouzi J, Meaux A, Müller N. Avoiding interactional conflict in dementia: the influence of gender styles in interaction. Journal of Language Aggression and Conflict. 2016;4:9–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  99. Hydén LC, Samuelsson C. "so they are not alive?": dementia, reality disjunctions and conversational strategies. Dementia (London). 2019;18:2662–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  100. Nguyen T, Li X. Understanding public-stigma and self-stigma in the context of dementia: a systematic review of the global literature. Dementia (London). 2020;19:148–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Michaelian JC, Mowszowski L, Guastella AJ, Henry JD, Duffy S, McCade D, et al. Theory of mind in mild cognitive impairment - relationship with limbic structures and behavioural change. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2019;25:1023–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Isaacowitz DM, Stanley JT. Bringing an ecological perspective to the study of aging and recognition of emotional facial expressions: past, current, and future methods. J Nonverbal Behav. 2011;35:261–78.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  103. Dawes S, Palmer B, Allison M, Ganiats T, Jeste D. Social desirability does not confound reports of wellbeing or of socio-demographic attitudes by older women. Ageing Soc. 2011;31:438–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  104. Kozma A, Stones MJ. Social desirability in measures of subjective well-being: a systematic evaluation. J Gerontol. 1987;42:56–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Webb AK, Jacobs-Lawson JM, Waddell EL. Older adults' perceptions of mentally ill older adults. Aging Ment Health. 2009;13:838–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  106. Ruppel SE, Jenkins WJ, Griffin JL, Kizer JB. Are they depressed or just old? A study of perceptions about the older adult suffering from depression. N Am J Psychol. 2010;12:31–42.

    Google Scholar 

  107. Murray J, Banerjee S, Byng R, Tylee A, Bhugra D, Macdonald A. Primary care professionals' perceptions of depression in older people: a qualitative study. Soc Sci Med. 2006;63:1363–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  108. Corcoran J, Brown E, Davis M, Pineda M, Kadolph J, Bell H. Depression in older adults: a meta-synthesis. J Gerontol Soc Work. 2013;56:509–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  109. Bowling A. Mode of questionnaire administration can have serious effects on data quality. J Public Health. 2005;27:281–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  110. Chen Y, Cui PY, Pan YY, Li YX, Waili N, Li Y. Association between housing environment and depressive symptoms among older people: a multidimensional assessment. BMC Geriatr. 2021;21:259.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  111. Smyth JD, Olson K, Millar MM. Identifying predictors of survey mode preference. Soc Sci Res. 2014;48:135–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  112. Palonen M, Kaunonen M, Åstedt-Kurki P. Exploring how to increase response rates to surveys of older people. Nurse Res. 2016;23:15–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  113. Greenwald HJ, Satow Y. A short social desirability scale. Psychol Rep. 1970;27:131–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  114. Bernardi RA. Validating research results when cronbach's alpha is below .70: a methodological procedure. Educ Psychol Meas Volume. 1994;54:766–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  115. Castle NG, Engberg J. Response formats and satisfaction surveys for elders. Gerontologist. 2004;44:358–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  116. Picco L, Lau YW, Pang S, Abdin E, Vaingankar JA, Chong SA, et al. Mediating effects of self-stigma on the relationship between perceived stigma and psychosocial outcomes among psychiatric outpatients: findings from a cross-sectional survey in Singapore. BMJ Open. 2017;7:e018228.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  117. Althubaiti A. Information bias in health research: definition, pitfalls, and adjustment methods. J Multidiscip Healthc. 2016;9:211–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  118. Paulhus DL. Measurement and control of response bias. In: Robinson JP, Shaver PR, Wrightsman LS, editors. Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes. San Diego: Academic Press; 1991. p. 17–59.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  119. Perinelli E, Gremigni P. Use of social desirability scales in clinical psychology: a systematic review. J Clin Psychol. 2016;72:534–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  120. McCrae RR, Costa PT. Social desirability scales: more substance than style. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1983;51:882–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  121. Holden RR. Socially desirable responding does moderate personality scale validity both in experimental and in nonexperimental contexts. Can J Behav Sci. 2007;39:184–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  122. Holden RR, Passey J. Socially desirable responding in personality assessment: not necessarily faking and not necessarily substance. Personal Individ Differ. 2010;49:446–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This study has no funding.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

PN designed the study, collected, analysed and interpreted the data. CA, SI and PDR collected and interpreted the data. DM and MC critically revised the manuscript for important intellectual content. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paola Nicolini.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was approved by the ethics committee of the Fondazione IRCCS Ca′ Granda Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico in Milan, Italy. All participants gave written informed consent to participate in the study. An overseeing mental health expert ruled that all adult patients and participants were capable of ethically and medically consenting to their participation in the research presented in this manuscript.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

The original online version of this article was revised: the authors reported that in the Results section, In the sentence beginning 'The lack of significant...', the term ' MCSDshow [?A3B2 h=0pt,128?]S ' should have read 'MCSDS'. Moreover, some small formatting errors have been corrected.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1:

Eight-item Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale (MCSDS).

Additional file 2: Tables S1.

and S2. PCA of the MCSDS in the NC and MCI groups.

Additional file 3: Table S3.

Binary logistic regression for the nature of “Kindness towards others” (formal versus substantive) as dependent variable.

Additional file 4: Table S4.

Test-retest reliability of the MCSDS after one month in a random sample of participants.

Additional file 5: Tables S5.

to S7. Spearman’s correlations between MCSDS and cognitive function scores in the NC and MCI groups and their comparison.

Additional file 6: Table S8.

Spearman’s correlations between MCSDS and GDS-s and STPI-TA scores in the NC and MCI groups and their comparison.

Additional file 7: Table S9.

Multiple linear regression with MCSDS scores as predictors of depressive and anxiety symptoms with cognitive status modelled as a dichotomous variable (NC vs MCI).

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Nicolini, P., Abbate, C., Inglese, S. et al. Socially desirable responding in geriatric outpatients with and without mild cognitive impairment and its association with the assessment of self-reported mental health. BMC Geriatr 21, 494 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-021-02435-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-021-02435-z

Keywords