- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Effects of Square Stepping Exercise on cognitive, physical, psychological, and group functioning in sedentary older adults: A center-based hybrid trial
BMC Geriatrics volume 24, Article number: 374 (2024)
Abstract
Background
The Square Stepping Exercise (SSE) is an exercise training program that integrates physical exercise and cognitive elements and can be conducted in a group setting. The potential of SSE in delaying cognitive decline in older adults is promising. However, the coronavirus pandemic has made it more difficult for older adults worldwide to exercise together in person. To address this issue, this study conducted a wholistic evaluation of the effects of a center-based hybrid SSE trial on cognitive, physical, psychological, and group functioning in sedentary older adults.
Methods
A total of 93 older adults (19 men, 74 women) participated in the study. Fifty-eight participants (9 men, 49 women) completed center-based hybrid SSE sessions over 12 weeks under coronavirus pandemic circumstances, whereas other 35 participants in the control group maintained their current level of daily activities. Cognitive functions focused on executive functions assessed by the Stroop Color-Word Test (inhibition) and the Trail Marking Test (TMT) (set-shifting). Psychological and group functioning were assessed by the Subjective Vitality Scale and the Physical Activity Group Environment Questionnaire. Physical function was evaluated by measuring gait speeds. A repeated ANOVA was conducted on the measured variables separately for the intervention and control groups to focus on the change of participant’s performance over data collection points.
Results
Outcomes of the Stroop Color-Word Test and the TMT revealed that the hybrid SSE was highly effective in improving executive function. Stroop performance (correct trials) was significantly improved in the incongruent condition, as well as both TMT-A and -B over the intervention period in the intervention group. The hybrid SSE was also beneficial to improve physical (gait speed at usual pace and at the maximum pace) as well as psychological functioning (subjective vitality). Furthermore, SSE participants reported increased engagement with the SSE task, social communication, and increased bonding and closeness with their group members through the hybrid SSE.
Conclusions
In this study, hybrid SSE was found to be effective in enhancing cognitive, physical, psychological, and group functioning in sedentary older adults. The findings of this study are crucial to provide older adults with a safer and efficient option to exercise.
The world’s population is aging much faster than in the past, and the population of people aged 60 years and above will be 2.1 billion across the world by 2050 [1]. To make the additional years of life positive for older people and for society, it is crucial to maintain physical and mental capacity of older adults. Karr et al. [2] have proposed that integrating both physical exercise and cognitive training in an intervention program may be more advantageous to maximize the benefits of training programs.
A training program that consists of both physical exercise and cognitive elements is the Square Stepping Exercise (SSE) [3]. SSE is a type of stepping exercise training that can be easily conducted in a group environment. The SSE consists of multi-direction movements, involving forward, backward, lateral and oblique step patterns. They are classified into 3 levels (elementary, intermediate, and advanced) based on the difficulty of the stepping patterns [3, 4, 15]. It was developed for older adults to do exercise indoor by overcoming difficulties faced when walking outdoors. Furthermore, SSE has the potential to support social interactions when it is done in a group environment [3].
Previous research showed that SSE was efficient to reduce fall risks in healthy community dwelling older adults [3,4,5] and to increase gait abilities and balancing of older adults with Parkinson’s disease [6]. Meta-analytic reviews [7, 8] also supported the effectiveness of SSE on fall prevention by enhancing balance [7, 8]. In addition to the physical benefits, the SSE was found to provide older adults with psychological benefits (e.g., improving depressive signs [9]) and cognitive benefits [10,11,12]. Teixeria et al. [10] found that a 14-week SSE program was effective in enhancing attention, global cognition, and mental flexibility [10], whereas other researchers found cognitive gains in executive function and memory [11, 12]).
The potential of SSE in delaying cognitive decline in older adults is promising [9,10,11]. However, the coronavirus pandemic has made it more difficult for older adults worldwide to exercise together in person. Individuals aged 60 years and over are more vulnerable to the coronavirus and at higher risk of developing serious conditions [13]. Considering the coronavirus pandemic, it was important to explore the option of the online SSE method for older adults. Kawabata et al. [15] invented protocols to conduct a home-based SSE trial online to increase the safety of participants. They reported that the online SSE was effective in improving executive function and group cohesion in sedentary young adults. Subsequently, Gan et al. [16] conducted a home-based online SSE with sedentary older adults and investigated its short-term effects on cognitive and group functioning by comparing with those of in-person SSE. They reported that the home-based online SSE was successful in enhancing executive function and group cohesion in sedentary older adults. However, the sample size of Gan et al’s study was very limited (online SSE: n = 7; in-person SSE: n = 17).
Given that the coronavirus pandemic has been stable but we need to live with the coronavirus, it would be useful to explore another option of the SSE approach for older adults. Therefore, the present study examined the effects of a hybrid SSE trial on cognitive, physical, psychological, and group functioning in sedentary older adults, hypothesizing significant positive outcomes in the intervention group compared to controls.
Methods
Participants and procedures
A total of 93 older adults (19 men, 74 women; Mage = 68.3 years, SD = 5.7) participated in the present study. The participants were healthy sedentary older adults without physical injuries or medical illness, according to the recruitment criteria indicated below. Fifty-eight participants (9 men, 49 women) in the intervention group were members of senior activity centers, who had never participated in SSE before. They completed hybrid SSE sessions at their centers. Thirty-five participants (10 men, 25 women) in the control group were recruited from online social media platforms. They were instructed to maintain their current level of physical activity during the study period without starting new activities. Participants’ characteristics are presented for each group in Table 1. Participants were recruited for intervention and control groups separately with the following inclusion criteria: (a) no psychiatric or neurological illnesses, (b) aged between 55 and 80 years old; and (c) exercise less than three times per week and not more than 30 min each time [16]. Consequently, participants were not randomly allocated to the groups in this study.
Ethical approval was obtained from an Institutional Review Board prior to data collection. Participation in the study was voluntary and informed consent was obtained from every participant. This study was conducted based on the approved guidelines and procedures over a 2-year period (from early 2022 to April 2023). At that time, the coronavirus situation was stable in Singapore and safe management measures were further eased by removing group size limits [14].
Exercise Protocol
Hybrid Square Stepping Exercise
The exercise sessions were conducted in hybrid mode through Zoom, an online video meeting software, twice a week over 12 weeks. Participants met in person at their respective senior activity center and participated in the SSE together in a group setting, but the exercise instruction was provided by an instructor online. As the participants were older adults, precautions against coronavirus were taken in this study. For example, the exercise sessions were limited to the members of each senior activity center. Participants also had to report to the staff whether they had symptoms. If they were feeling unwell, they were advised to stay home. All SSE sessions were scheduled in the morning.
SSE was performed on a thin felt mat that is 2.5 m in length (100 cm × 250 cm) and partitioned into 40 squares (25 cm each). At the start of each round, a certified trainer demonstrated a stepping pattern online to participants by following Kawabata et al.’s study [15]. Participants were asked to remember the pattern shown on the screen. Then, they stepped from one end of the mat to the other by following the stepping pattern. They took turns to execute the pattern presented in each round. Once finished, participants returned to the initial position by walking outside the mat. Positive social interactions were promoted during the SSE session through social communications with verbal encouragements and engagement of ‘air’ high-fives between participants on completion of trials.
Measures
Participants in both intervention and control groups were asked to complete the following measures (except for group cohesion) before, in the middle, and after the 12-week study period. The measure of group cohesion was administered only to the participants in the intervention group before and after the intervention.
Executive function
The Stroop Color-Word Test and the Trail Making Test (TMT) was administered using Inquisit 5 [17] to measure speed of cognitive processing, cognitive flexibility and inhibition [18]. In the Stroop Color-Word Test, cognitive interference (inhibition) was simulated by asking participants to name the color of the word and not read the word itself. The test consists of three different conditions: (i) congruent trials; (ii) incongruent trials; and (iii) control trials [19]. The Stroop congruent and incongruent conditions are the indices of basic information processing and executive function, respectively [19]. The TMT comprised two parts (A and B). Part A (TMT-A) is associated with visuomotor processing speed and Part B (TMT-B) is considered to reflect working memory and task-switching ability [17, 20]. The difference in time taken to complete TMT-B and the time to complete TMT-A (i.e., B– A) was calculated to measure executive function [19, 20].
Group cohesion
The Physical Activity Group Environment Questionnaire (PAGEQ) [22] was employed to assess participant’s experience of cohesion in their physical activity groups. The PAGEQ is an instrument with 21 items, consisting of the four subscales: Individual Attractions to the Group-Task (ATG-T: personal participation in the group task), Individual Attractions to the Group-Social (ATG-S: personal approval and social communication with the group), Group Integration-Task (GI-T: the bonding and closeness that exists within the group as a whole around its joint task), and Group Integration-Social (GI-S: the bonding and closeness that exist within the group as a whole around social matters). Participants were requested to show the degree to which they agreed with the statement of each item by using a 9-point Likert-type scale, which ranged from 1 (very strongly disagree) to 9 (very strongly agree).
Subjective vitality
The Subjective Vitality Scale (SVS) [23] is a self-report instrument that is designed to evaluate state and trait feelings of energy and vitality. When people are vital, they are more active and productive, coping better with stress and challenges, and have greater mental health and wellness [24]. The SVS has been extensively used in the research on health, motivation, and well-being [25]. The five-item trait-version of the SVS was employed in the current study as it was found to be a sensitive measure in Kawabata et al. [25]. Participants were requested to indicate the degree to which the statement of each item was true for them “in general in their life” based on a 7-point Likert-type scale, which ranged from 1 (not at all true) to 7 (very true).
Physical tests
A walk and a single-leg balance test were used to measure participant’s physical functioning abilities. For the gait test, participants had to walk along a straight 11-metre walkway at their (1) usual pace once, and (2) maximum pace twice (the faster measurement was taken) [26]. In the balance test, participants were asked to balance on either their left or right leg with their eyes open and hands crossed over their chest; the time they were able to maintain balance in this position was measured [27]. The time begins when they raise their foot and ends when participants attempt to regain their balance by (1) using their arms, (2) placing their raised foot down, or (3) shifting the weight bearing foot. The test also ended when they were able to maintain their balance for the maximum duration of 30s.
Data Analysis
In preliminary analysis, participants in the control group were found to be significantly younger and more educated than the intervention group (reported in the preliminary results in the Results section later). Therefore, a repeated ANOVA was conducted on the measured variables separately for the intervention and control groups to focus on the change of participant’s performance over data collection time points. A repeated MANOVA was conducted on the PAGEQ subscale scores, which were only applicable to the intervention group. When pairwise comparison was performed as part of the post hoc test, Bonferroni-adjusted p-values (0.017 [0.05/3]) were used to identify statistical difference between time points (baseline, middle, post) by controlling the overall Type I error rate.
Results
Preliminary analysis
Descriptive statistics of participant’s age and frequencies of participant’s education level are presented in Table 1. Participants in the control group (M = 68.3, SD = 5.71) were significantly younger than those in the intervention group (M = 70.5, SD = 4.61; t[91] = 5.55, p <.001, d = 1.16). Furthermore, participants in the control group were more educated, compared to those in the intervention group (χ2 = 36.62, p <.001; total estimated years of education: t[91] = 6.44, p <.001, d = 1.38). According to these results, statistical comparison between groups would be questionable. Instead, focusing on the change of participant’s performance over time in each group was considered more appropriate and meaningful.
Executive function
Stroop Performance in the incongruent condition
Only correct trials were included in the reaction time (RT) analyses as it was difficult to interpret RT of incorrect trials. Descriptive statistics of RT (in milliseconds) and accuracy of Stroop performance in the incongruent condition are presented in Table 2; Fig. 1. The main effect of data collection time points on RT was significant in the intervention group (F[2,144] = 9.07, p <.001, ηp2 = 0.14), but not in the control group (F[2,68] = 2.26, p =.112, ηp2 = 0.06). Specifically for the intervention group, the RT became faster from baseline to middle (pre: M = 2598.239, SD = 1281.522; middle: M = 2183.263, SD = 765.458; p =.011, d = 0.393), as well as from the baseline to the post (post: M = 2105.745, SD = 754.787; p =.002, d = 0.468). The main effect of data collection time points on accuracy was also significant in the intervention group (F[2,144] = 5.37, p =.006, ηp2 = 0.09), but not in the control group (F[2,68] = 2.88, p =.063, ηp2 = 0.08). Post hoc tests revealed that accuracy of correct Stroop performance was significantly improved from baseline to post in the intervention group (pre: M = 0.785, SD = 0.275; post: M = 0.899, SD = 0.161; p =.013, d = 0.506).
Trial making test
Descriptive statistics of TMT-A and TMT-B scores are presented in Table 2; Fig. 2. The main effect of data collection time points on the TMT-A duration was significant in the intervention group (F[2,144] = 8.41, p <.001, ηp2 = 0.13), but not in the control group (F[2,68] = 1.01, p =.369, ηp2 = 0.06). Post hoc tests indicated that the intervention group took a significantly shorter time to complete the TMT-A from baseline to post (pre: M = 76362.53, SD = 28295.07; post: M = 64680.26, SD = 23530.48; p <.001, d = 0.449). The main effect of data collection time points on the TMT-B duration was significant in the intervention group (F[2,86] = 8.93, p <.001, ηp2 = 0.17) as well as in the control group (F[2,66] = 4.20, p =.019, ηp2 = 0.11). Post hoc tests revealed that the intervention group took a significantly shorter time to complete the TMT-B from baseline to middle (pre: M = 132937.16, SD = 64970.10; middle: M = 112067.55, SD = 64942.19; p <.009, d = 0.321) as well as from baseline to post (post: M = 105212.84, SD = 63664.67; p =.001, d = 0.431). Although the control group took a significantly shorter duration to complete the TMT-B from baseline to middle (pre: M = 88361.24, SD = 37569.05; middle: M = 75232.74, SD = 29214.66, p =.011, d = 0.390), further improvement was not observed between baseline and post (post: M = 74893.97, SD = 37414.43, p =.099). The main effect of data collection time points on the difference in performance between TMT-B and TMT-A (B – A) was significant in the intervention group (F[2, 86] = 3.62, p =.031, ηp2 = 0.08), but not in the control group (F[2,66] = 1.59, p =.212, ηp2 = 0.05). Post hoc tests indicated that TMT-B minus TMT-A (B– A) got significantly smaller from baseline to post (pre: M = 61284.09, SD = 56118.49; post: M = 43804.98, SD = 51192.29; p <.05, d = 0.325).
Gait speeds
Descriptive statistics of gait speeds are presented in Table 2; Fig. 3. The main effect of data collection time points on gait speed at usual pace and at maximum pace was significant in the intervention group (usual pace: F[2,114] = 7.51, p <.001, ηp2 = 0.12; maximum pace: F[2,114] = 9.71, p <.011, ηp2 = 0.15), but not significant in the control group (usual: F[2,68] = 3.30, p =.08, ηp2 = 0.09; maximum: F[2,68] = 0.70, p =.50, ηp2 = 0.02). Post hoc tests indicated that intervention group’s gait speed at usual pace (pre: M = 1.14, SD = 0.20; post: M = 1.22, SD = 0.17; p <.001, d = 0.431) as well as at maximum pace (pre: M = 1.46, SD = 0.21; post: M = 1.53, SD = 0.20; p <.001, d = 0.341) were significantly improved from baseline to post.
Subjective vitality
The SVS scores in the intervention and control groups are presented in Table 2; Fig. 4. The main effect of data collection time points on the SVS scores was significant in the intervention group (F[2,114] = 4.73, p =.011, ηp2 = 0.77), but not significant in the control group (F[2,68] = 2.99, p =.743, ηp2 = 0.01). Post-hoc tests revealed that intervention group’s vitality score significantly increased from baseline to post (pre: M = 5.56, SD = 0.92; post: M = 5.83, SD = 0.98; p =.019, d = 0.284).
Group Cohesion
The averaged PAGEQ subscale scores in the intervention group are shown in Table 2; Fig. 5. A repeated MANOVA on the PAGEQ subscale scores indicated that the main effect of data collection time points on all the four subscale scores were significant (ATG-T: F[2,114] = 18.47, p <.001, ηp2 = 0.30; ATG-S: F[2,114] = 16.06, p <.001, ηp2 = 0.22; GI-T: F[2,114] = 12.71, p <.001, ηp2 = 0.18; GI-S: F[2,114] = 11.55, p <.001, ηp2 = 0.18). Post-hoc tests revealed that all the scores significantly increased from baseline to the middle as well as to the post in the intervention group (see Fig. 5).
Discussion
To ensure the safety and well-being of older adults, it is crucial to have alternative options of conducting SSE in different modes (e.g., online, hybrid, and in-person modes) for older adults and examine the efficacy of the approaches. To this end, the present study investigated the effects of a hybrid SSE on cognitive, physical, psychological, and group functioning in sedentary older adults.
Outcomes of cognitive tests (the Stroop Color-Word Test and the TMT) revealed that the hybrid SSE was highly effective to improve executive function that included working memory, inhibition, and cognitive flexibility [28]. The intervention group showed significant improvement in reaction time and accuracy of correct Stroop performance in the incongruent condition, as well as the TMT-B minus TMT-A (B– A) in the present study. This finding is consistent with several previous studies on in-person SSE and pilot online SSE [10,11,12, 15–16]. These results suggest that the cognitive challenges included in SSE were properly implemented in the hybrid SSE. In the SSE program, participants need to pay close attention to the instructor’s demonstrations to remember stepping patterns and execute those steps subsequently. They also need to inhibit and modify their natural walking behaviors to finish the demonstrated complex steps accurately [11, 15]. The inhibitory control and fast information processing speed incorporated in the hybrid SSE are useful to prevent falling in daily life settings [8, 11].
Results of physical tests indicated that the hybrid SSE was also beneficial to improve physical functions, such as gait speed at usual pace and at the maximum pace. These results replicated findings of previous in-person SSE studies [5, 6]. Furthermore, the hybrid SSE was effective to enhance subjective vitality, which confirmed the result from a previous pilot study on the online SSE [16].
It is of note that the perception of cohesion by the intervention group in the hybrid SSE were significantly enhanced over the 12 weeks. In the present study, all four PAGEQ subscale scores significantly increased over the intervention period. This means that participants in the intervention group perceived increased engagement and communication with group members in the hybrid SSE task. They also indicated increased feelings of bonding and closeness within the group over the 12 weeks. These results are consistent with some previous studies on in-person SSE [11–12] as well as online SSE [15–16]. The consistent findings confirmed that it is possible to promote different aspects of group cohesion in the hybrid SSE sessions.
Conclusions
In the present study, SSE was conducted in a hybrid mode amid the COVID-19 pandemic and found to be effective to improve cognitive, physical, psychological, and group functioning in sedentary older adults. These results, which are consistent with previous studies on in-person SSE and pilot online SSE [10,11,12, 15–16], confirmed that the hybrid SSE is helpful for older adults to gain multiple benefits through the activity. The novel hybrid method and results are the strengths of the present study. Despite the strong points, there are also limitations to the study. For instance, a passive control group was employed in this study. Our original plan was to use an active control group who take part in brisk walking. However, it was impossible to implement the plan due to the challenging circumstances caused by the COVID-19 pandemic. As a passive control group was recruited separately from the intervention group, significant differences in demographics (e.g., age and education) were observed between intervention and control groups. We were thus not able to compare the effects of the hybrid SSE between the groups directly. This is another limitation of the current study and future randomized control trials are encouraged.
Given the coronavirus pandemic circumstance, it was valuable to explore the hybrid SSE approach for older adults. In future research, the multiple beneficial effects of the hybrid SSE for older adults or other populations (e.g., patients [29]) can be further examined by employing an active control group.
Data availability
The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
References
World Health Organization. (2022, October 1). Ageing and health. [Retrieved on September 10, 2023]. https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/ageing-and-health.
Karr JE, Areshenkoff CN, Rast P, Garcia-Barrera MA. An empirical comparison of the therapeutic benefits of physical exercise and cognitive training on the executive functions of older adults: a meta-analysis of controlled trials. Neuropsychology. 2014;28:829.
Shigematsu R, Okura T. A novel exercise for improving lower-extremity functional fitness in the elderly. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2006;18:242–8.
Shigematsu R, Okura T, Sakai T, Rantanen T. Square-stepping exercise versus strength and balance training for fall risk factors. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2008a;20:19–24.
Shigematsu R, Okura T, Nakagaichi M, Tanaka K, Sakai T, Kitazumi S, Rantanen T. Square-stepping exercise and fall risk factors in older adults: a single-blind, randomized controlled trial. Journals Gerontol Ser A: Biol Sci Med Sci. 2008b;63:76–82.
Ravichandran H, Janakiraman B, Yitayeh A, Sundaram S, Fisseha B. Effectiveness of square stepping exercise among subjects with Parkinson’s disease: a pilot randomized controlled trial. J Geriatric Mental Health. 2017;4:54057.
Fisseha B, Janakiraman B, Yitayeh A, Ravichandran H. Effect of square stepping exercise for older adults to prevent fall and injury related to fall: systematic review and meta-analysis of current evidences. J Exerc Rehabilitation. 2017;13:23.
Nokham R, Kitisri C. Effect of square-stepping exercise on balance in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Phys Fit Sports Med. 2017;6:183–90.
Pereira JR, Gobbi S, Teixeira CV, Nascimento CM, Corazza DI, Vital TM, Shigematsu R. Effects of square-stepping exercise on balance and depressive symptoms in older adults. Motriz: Revista De Educacao Fisica. 2014;20:454–60.
Teixeira CV, Gobbi S, Pereira JR, Vital TM, Hernandéz SS, Shigematsu R, Gobbi LT. Effects of square-stepping exercise on cognitive functions of older people. Psychogeriatrics. 2013;13:148–56.
Shigematsu R, Okura T, Nakagaichi M, Nakata Y. Effects of exercise program requiring attention, memory and imitation on cognitive function in elderly persons: a non-randomized pilot study. J Gerontol Geriatric Res. 2014;3:147.
Noma A, Uchida R, Kurosaki T, Numao S, Nakagaichi M. Effects of the Square-Stepping Exercise program on physical fitness and cognitive function in elderly. Japanese J Phys Fit Sports Med. 2020;69:393–400.
World Health Organization. (2020, October 12). Coronavirus disease (COVID-19). https://www.who.int/emergencies/diseases/novel-coronavirus-2019/question-and-answers-hub/q-a-detail/coronavirus-disease-covid-19.
Government of Singapore. (2022, October 10). [Updated] Further easing of community measures. https://www.gov.sg/article/further-easing-of-community-measures.
Kawabata M, Gan SR, Goh G, Omar SAB, Oh ITF, Wee WQ, Okura T. Acute effects of Square stepping Exercise on cognitive and social functions in sedentary young adults: a home-based online trial. BMC Sports Sci Med Rehabilitation. 2021;13:82.
Gan SR, Low K, Okura T, Kawabata M. Short-term effects of Square stepping Exercise on cognitive and social functions in sedentary older adults: a home-based online trial. ACPES J Phys Educ Sport Health. 2022;2:70–7.
Inquisit 5 [Computer software]. (2016). Retrieved from https://www.millisecond.com. Accessed 7 Jan.
Sánchez-Cubillo I, Periáñez JA, Adrover-Roig D, Rodríguez-Sánchez JM, Ríos-Lago M, Tirapu J, Barceló F. Construct validity of the trail making test: role of task-switching, working memory, inhibition/interference control, and visuomotor abilities. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15:438–50.
Etnier JL. Cognitive measures related to exercise and physical activity. In: Tenenbaum G, Eklund R, Kamata A, editors. Measurement in sport and exercise psychology. Champaign, IL, USA: Human Kinetics; 2012. pp. 179–89.
McMorris T. (2016). History of research into the acute exercise-cognition interaction: A cognitive psychology approach. McMorris, T, editor, Exercise-cognition interaction: Neuroscience perspectives. Academic Press: New York, USA.
Arbuthnott K, Frank J. Trail making test, part B as a measure of executive control: validation using a set-switching paradigm. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2000;22:518–28.
Estabrooks P, Carron A. The physical activity group environment questionnaire: an instrument for the assessment of cohesion in exercise classes. Group Dynamics Theory Res Pract. 2000;4:230–43.
Ryan RM, Frederick CM. On energy, personality and health: subjective vitality as a dynamic reflection of well-being. J Pers. 1997;65:529–65.
Kasser VG, Ryan RM. The relation of psychological needs for autonomy and relatedness to vitality, wellbeing, and mortality in a nursing home. J Appl Soc Psychol. 1999;29:935–54.
Kawabata M, Yamazaki F, Guo DW, Chatzisarantis NLD. Advancement of the subjective vitality scale: examining alternative measurement models for Japanese and singaporeans. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2017;27:1793–800.
Taniguchi, Y., Kitamura, A., Seino, S., Murayama, H., Amano, H., Nofuji, Y.,… Shinkai,S. (2017). Gait performance trajectories and incident disabling dementia among community-dwelling older Japanese. Journal of the American Medical Directors Association, 18, e13-e20.
Springer BA, Marin R, Cyhan T, Roberts H, Gill NW. Normative values for the unipedal stance test with eyes open and closed. J Geriatr Phys Ther. 2007;30:8–15.
Diamond A. Executive functions. Ann Rev Psychol. 2013;64:135–68.
Túbero GS, Gobbi S, Teixeira CVL, Pereira JR, Shigematsu R, Canonici AP. Effects of square stepping exercise in patients with sequel of cerebrovascular accident. Fisioterapia em Movimento. 2014;27:229–37.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank all participants for their time. Without their voluntary participations, this study would not have been possible. Appreciation also goes to ActiveSG for their assistance in the coordination of the sessions, and the research assistants who assisted in some of the screening and assessments conducted for the participants.
Funding
The study was financially supported by Ministry of Education Academic Research Fund Tier 2 Grant (Project No.: MOE2019-T2-1-019). The views expressed in this paper are the authors’ and do not necessarily represent the views of Singapore Ministry of Education. The publication cost of this paper was supported by a research fund MK had received (Rikkyo University Special Fund for Research).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
This study was conducted when MK was at National Institute of Education, Nanyang Technological University. MK is the Co-Lead principal investigator of the funded project below and SRG is the project manager. MK was in charge of the present intervention study and developed and designed the study. MK took a lead role in drafting the manuscript with supports from SRG. ACSH is the Lead principal investigator of the following funded project and contributed to the initial study design, set up of the cognitive battery and administration, and input on data analysis. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
Ethical approval for the present study was gained from The Institutional Review Board at Nanyang Technological University (NTU), Singapore (Ref: IRB-2020-04-014). All participants gave their informed consent to participate in the study. This study was carried out consistent with the guidelines and procedures involving human subjects, which were approved by the Institutional Review Board.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
About this article
Cite this article
Kawabata, M., Gan, S.R. & Shen-Hsing, A.C. Effects of Square Stepping Exercise on cognitive, physical, psychological, and group functioning in sedentary older adults: A center-based hybrid trial. BMC Geriatr 24, 374 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-024-04904-7
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-024-04904-7