Skip to main content

Determining optimal cutoff scores of Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument to identify dementia and mild cognitive impairment in Taiwan

Abstract

Background

The early detection of dementia depends on efficient methods for the assessment of cognitive capacity. Existing cognitive screening tools are ill-suited to the differentiation of cognitive status, particularly when dealing with early-stage impairment.

Methods

The study included 8,979 individuals (> 50 years) with unimpaired cognitive functions, mild cognitive impairment (MCI), or dementia. This study sought to determine optimal cutoffs values for the Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument (CASI) aimed at differentiating between individuals with or without dementia as well as between individuals with or without mild cognitive impairment. Cox proportional hazards models were used to evaluate the value of CASI tasks in predicting conversion from MCI to all-cause dementia, dementia of Alzheimer’s type (DAT), or to vascular dementia (VaD).

Results

Our optimized cutoff scores achieved high accuracy in differentiating between individuals with or without dementia (AUC = 0.87—0.93) and moderate accuracy in differentiating between CU and MCI individuals (AUC = 0.67 – 0.74). Among individuals without cognitive impairment, scores that were at least 1.5 × the standard deviation below the mean scores on CASI memory tasks were predictive of conversion to dementia within roughly 2 years after the first assessment (all-cause dementia: hazard ratio [HR] = 2.81 – 3.53; DAT: 1.28 – 1.49; VaD: 1.58). Note that the cutoff scores derived in this study were lower than those reported in previous studies.

Conclusion

Our results in this study underline the importance of establishing optimal cutoff scores for individuals with specific demographic characteristics and establishing profiles by which to guide CASI analysis.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Determining the cognitive status of elderly individuals can be exceedingly difficult when dealing with early-stage dementia [1]. Cognitive assessments play a key role in the diagnosis of dementia and its prodromal stage, mild cognitive impairment (MCI) [2, 3]; however, comprehensive neuropsychological assessments are time-consuming and not universally applicable [3, 4]. Cognitive screening tests (CSTs) are a cost-effective alternative to assessing cognitive function. The Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument (CASI) has been widely used CST in research; however, its low sensitivity leads to a high false negative rate [3] in the early detection of dementia [5, 6].

When dealing with dementia or MCI, detection sensitivity could be affected by any number of factors [7, 8], including inter- and intra-study variations in demographic characteristics [4]. Researchers have listed education level and age as important demographic variable capable of affecting cognitive test scores [9, 10]. Accurate determinations of cognitive status depend on optimal cutoff scores for individuals with specific demographic characteristics. Furthermore, most CSTs rely on global scores, despite the fact that individuals with MCI are prone to cognitive impairment in specific cognitive domains rather than in general cognitive functioning [2, 11].

In the current study, we sought to derive optimal cutoff scores for CASI among a large sample of Taiwanese individuals (n = 8,979). We also sought to derive cutoff scores for screening tests used to predict MCI or dementia. Finally, we derived cutoff scores for various CASI subtests based on the mean score and standard deviation. This study was based on three hypotheses: (1) CASI scores can be used to differentiate individuals according to cognitive status as unimpaired, MCI, and dementia; and (2) CASI scores can be used to predict the conversion of unimpaired individuals to MCI or dementia.

Methods

Participants

Study subjects were selected from a longitudinal dementia registry dataset established for the History-based Artificial Intelligent Clinical Dementia Diagnostic System (HAICDDS) by the Show Chwan Healthcare System, which covers all of Taiwan except the eastern region [12,13,14,15,16]. The objective in establishing HAICDDS was to enable regular examinations (serum tests, cognitive assessment tests, and laboratory examinations) to facilitate the early detection and prevention of dementia. At present, the dataset includes 10,526 participants, comprising 20,018 data points. We selected 8,979 individuals over 50 years of age who completed CASI (Fig. 1). Among this cohort, 1,629 individuals were followed for an average of 2 \(\pm\) 1.05 years (range: 0.5 – 6 years) to assess the predictive value of the respective cognitive task scores on conversion to dementia. These individuals were diagnosed with incident dementia of Alzheimer’s type (DAT) or vascular dementia (VaD) during follow-up sessions in accordance with criteria proposed by the National Institute on Aging and Alzheimer’s Association (NIA-AA) [17], the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS), and the Association Internationale pour la Recherche et l'Enseignement en Neurosciences (AIREN) [18, 19]. Computed tomography (CT) scans or magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans captured using a Siemens 3 T scanner were used to aid in the diagnosis and excluding apparent etiologies other than degenerative or cerebrovascular conditions. MCI patients and CU individuals without incident dementia were categorized into AD or vascular origin groups based on the clinical diagnosis criteria put forth by previous studies [20, 21] or by retrospectively excluding other etiologies, respectively.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Selection process of study subjects. DAT: Dementia of Alzheimer’s type. HAICDDS: History-Based Artificial Intelligent Clinical Dementia Diagnostic System. LBD: Lewy body dementia. MCI Mild cognitive impairment. PD Parkinson’s disease. VaD Vascular dementia

Note that the subjects excluded from the prediction study tended to be older (77.56 \(\pm\) 8.59 years), male (56.70%), and possessing a lower education level (4.30 \(\pm\) 4.12 years; p < 0.05). Global cognitive function among participants was assessed by clinical neuropsychologists using CASI [22] and the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) [23, 24]. Note also that this study used cutoff scores suggested by Lin et al. (2002) for the determination of cognitive deficits. The activities of daily living (ADL) were assessed using the History-based Artificial Intelligent ADL questionnaire (HAI-ADL) [25]. A diagnosis of cognitively unimpaired (CU) required a global Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR)-sum of boxes (CDR-SB) score of 0 or 0.5 [26, 27] and normally performance on CASI. Individuals with a CDR-SB score of 0.5 were assigned to the CU group, due to the fact that most individuals with a score of this level maintain cognitive stability [27]. The inclusion of a clinical sample in this study suggests that some of the subtle changes indicative of cognitive decline may not have been captured by the tests in this study. MCI was diagnosed in accordance with criteria proposed in previous studies [28], and an operational determination of MCI was indicated by a CASI score below the cutoff score, a normal performance on HAI-ADL, and a CDR score of at least 0.5. A diagnosis of dementia was based on NIA-AA criteria [17], and an operational determination of impaired cognitive function or ADL was indicated by performance below the cutoffs for CASI and HAI-ADL [22]. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Show Chwan Memorial Hospital (SCMH_IRB1081006). Informed consent was waived because of the retrospective nature of the study and the analysis used anonymous clinical data.

CASI

CASI comprises 25 tasks selected from the Hasegawa Dementia Screening Scale, the Mini-Mental State Examination, and the Modified Mini-Mental State Test [11, 25]. The CASI assesses nine cognitive domains, including long-term memory, short-term memory, attention, mental manipulation, orientation, abstract thinking, language, visuospatial construction, and verbal fluency. Long-term memory was assessed in terms of general knowledge (e.g., “mooncakes are typical desserts during the celebration of which holiday?”) (max score = 10). Short-term memory was assessed by having the participant repeat three words read aloud by the examiner (max score = 12). Attention was assessed by having the participant repeat two syntactically complex sentences (max score = 8). Mental manipulation was assessed by asking participants to subtract 7 from 100 consecutively 5 times and then repeat digits read as read aloud by the examiner or in reverse order (max score = 10). Orientation was assessed by asking the participants about their current location, date, and time (max score = 18). Abstract thinking was assessed by having participants describe the similarities between two objects (e.g., a shrimp and a fish; a table and a chair) (max score = 12). Language skills were assessed by having participants write five common Chinese characters and perform confrontational naming of body parts and common objects (max score = 10). Visuospatial construction was assessed by having the participant copy two partially overlapping pentagons (max score = 10). Verbal fluency was assessed by having the participants name as many four-legged animals as they could in a 30-s period (max score = 10). Note that in the short-term memory task, three points were awarded if the participant spontaneously recalled each of the target words, two points were awarded if the participant recalled the target words after category cueing (e.g., “something to wear” for hat), and one point was awarded if the participant recalled the target words after the examiner provided three choices (e.g., “shoes, hat, socks”).

In a previous study, Lin et al. (2002) established cut-off scores to differentiate between individuals with dementia and those deemed cognitively healthy in a group of 2,096 elderly Taiwanese. The cognitively healthy individuals were recruited from Kinmen County and the patients with dementia were recruited from Taipei. The cut-off scores were as follows: illiterate individuals (49/50), individuals with an education below the elementary level (67/68); and individuals with an elementary level education or higher (79/80). These scores resulted in sensitivity of 0.83–0.89 and specificity of 0.85–0.91.

Statistical analysis

We compared demographic and clinical characteristics among CU individuals and those with MCI or dementia using multiple one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) or chi-square tests. We sought to establish optimal cutoffs for four specific demographic subgroups stratified according to the demographical distributions of the sample and Taiwanese population [29]: younger individuals (< 75 years) with a relatively low education level (< 6 years of education; n = 698), younger individuals with higher education level (\(\ge\) 6 years of education; n = 3,068), older individuals (\(\ge\) 75 years) with a relatively low education level (n = 3,015) and older individuals with higher education level (n = 2,198). Further a priori multiple regression analysis revealed that gender did not have a significant effect on CASI scores (p = 0.30 – 0.91). Independent sample t-tests and chi-square tests were used to analyze demographic and clinical characteristics. Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves were used to assess the performances of CASI cut-off scores in differentiating among participants in terms of cognitive status. CASI scores compared individuals with and without dementia as well as individuals with or without mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Optimal cut-off scores were derived using the Youden Index as follows: maximum = sensitivity + specificity – 1. In deriving the cutoff score for suspected cognitive impairment, we adopted a conventional criterion suggested in previous studies; i.e., 1.5 × the standard deviation below the reference or normative mean on the total score and the score for each of the cognitive domains [28, 30]. In the longitudinally predictive study, we adopted the criterion for determination of predictive values of scores below the cutoff on conversion to dementia. The distribution of scores in the CU group was used as a reference. Cox proportional hazard regression models were used to compare the risk of incident dementia as a function of global score or the scores for the various cognitive domains, after controlling for the effects of demographic variables. Schoenfeld residuals were examined using visual scanning to assess whether the proportional hazard (PH) assumption for Cox regression was violated [31]. The random scattering of residuals indicated that the PH assumption was not violated.

Results

Demographics and clinical characteristics

Table 1 presents the demographic and clinical characteristics in each group. The average age was higher in the dementia group than in the CU and MCI groups, in the following subsamples: total (F 2, 8976 = 959.778, p < .001), older (illiterate: F 2, 2161 = 78.48, p < .001; older-low education: F 2, 3014 = 87.817, p < .001; older-high education: F 2, 2197 = 109.615, p < .001), and younger/ high education subsamples (F 2, 3067 = 22.209, p < .001). The average age of subjects was higher in the MCI group than in the CU group, including the total sample and younger-high education and older-high education subsamples. The proportion of females in the dementia group was higher in the CU and MCI groups: total sample (χ2 df = 2, n = 8979 = 46.69, p < .001). The proportion of males was higher in all younger subsample groups (more educated: χ2 df = 2, n = 3068 = 11.89, p = 0.003; less educated sample: χ2 df = 2, n = 313 = 6.23, p = 0.044), except the illiterate subsample (p = 0.047, no difference in post-hoc comparison). The education level was higher in the CU group than in the MCI and dementia groups in the following subsamples: total (F 2, 8976 = 533.49, p < .001) and younger/ high education (F 2, 3067 = 49.42, p < .001). In the older-high education subsample, the mean education level was higher in the CU group than in the dementia group (F 2, 2197 = 3.26, p = 0.04).

Table 1 Demographical and clinical characteristics of participants

Among the CU individuals who underwent follow-up assessments (n = 526), 25.5% converted to dementia (n = 134). Among the MCI individuals who underwent follow-up assessments (n = 1,103), 39.7% converted to dementia (n = 438). Note that the follow-up duration of MCI converters was higher than that of MCI non-converters. Among MCI converters, the proportion of females was higher (χ2 df = 1, n =1,103 = 4.76, p = 0.03), the education level was lower (t = 4.31, p < 0.001), the average age was higher (t = 5.13, p < 0.001), and scores on CDR-Sum of Boxes were higher (t = 4.00, p < 0.001). In the CU group, we observed no significant demographics differences between converters and non-converters (p = 0.06 – 0.58) (Supplementary Table 1).

Differentiating cognitive status based on total CASI scores

Dementia vs. no dementia

Table 2 lists the effectiveness of CASI in discriminating between individuals with or without dementia and MCI vs. CU. In discriminating between individuals with or without dementia, the CASI cut-off scores optimized for age were as follows: ≤ 75 years (72/73: AUC = 0.92, sensitivity = 0.83, specificity = 0.84, p < 0.001) and > 75 years (55/56: AUC = 0.89, sensitivity = 0.82, specificity = 0.80, p < 0.001). The CASI cut-off scores optimized for education level were as follows: illiterate (47/48: AUC = 0.90, sensitivity = 0.84, specificity = 0.81, p < 0.001), ≤ 6 years of education (55/56: AUC = 0.88, sensitivity = 0.84, specificity = 0.77, p < 0.001), and > 6 years of education (72/73: AUC = 0.93, sensitivity = 0.84, specificity = 0.86, p < 0.001). The CASI cut-off scores optimized for younger subjects were as follows: younger-illiterate (50/51: AUC = 0.89, sensitivity = 0.88, specificity = 0.76, p < 0.001), younger-low education (60/61: AUC = 0.87, sensitivity = 0.83, specificity = 0.91, p < 0.001), and younger-high education (76/77: AUC = 0.92, sensitivity = 0.84, specificity = 0.86, p < 0.001). The CASI cut-off scores optimized for older subjects were as follows: older-illiterate (42/43: AUC = 0.90, sensitivity = 0.88, specificity = 0.75, p < 0.001), older-low education (53/54: AUC = 0.87, sensitivity = 0.85, specificity = 0.75, p < 0.001), and older-high education (63/64: AUC = 0.91, sensitivity = 0.87, specificity = 0.78, p < 0.001).

Table 2 AUC and cutoff points of CASI

CU vs. MCI

In discriminating individuals with CU or MCI, the CASI cut-off scores optimized for age were as follows: ≤ 75 years of age (81/82: AUC = 0.69, sensitivity = 0.78, specificity = 0.52, p < 0.001) and > 75 years (72/73: AUC = 0.73, sensitivity = 0.72, specificity = 0.66, p < 0.001). The CASI cut-off scores optimized for education level were as follows: illiterate (64/65: AUC = 0.74, sensitivity = 0.67, specificity = 0.71, p < 0.001), ≤ 6 years of education (68/69: AUC = 0.73, sensitivity = 0.78, specificity = 0.62, p < 0.001), and > 6 years of education (81/82: AUC = 0.71, sensitivity = 0.78, specificity = 0.54, p < 0.001). The CASI cut-off scores optimized for younger subjects were as follows: younger-illiterate (64/65; AUC = 0.72, sensitivity = 0.81, specificity = 0.56, p < 0.001), younger-low education (68/69: AUC = 0.69, sensitivity = 0.84, specificity = 0.47, p < 0.001), and younger high education (83/84: AUC = 0.67, sensitivity = 0.78, specificity = 0.48, p < 0.001). The CASI cut-off scores optimized for older subjects were as follows: older-illiterate (62/63: AUC = 0.73, sensitivity = 0.67, specificity = 0.68, p < 0.001), older-low education (67/68: AUC = 0.75, sensitivity = 0.74, specificity = 0.69, p < 0.001), and older-high education (77/78: AUC = 0.73, sensitivity = 0.76, specificity = 0.62, p < 0.001).

Risk of conversion to dementia

Table 3 lists the CASI cutoff scores derived for each of the cognitive domains based on the degree of dispersion beyond 1.5 × the SD of mean scores in the CU group. After controlling for the effects of demographics, we determined that performance values below the cutoff scores for the long-term memory subtest (B = 1.26, hazard ratio [HR] = 3.53, CI = 2.12 – 5.89) and short-term memory subtest (B = 1.03, HR = 2.81, CI = 1.81 – 4.37) were predictive of conversion to dementia. None of the other subtests were predictive of conversion (p = 0.13 – 0.96) (Fig. 2 and Supplementary Fig. 1).

Table 3 The cutoff scores for CASI subscales
Fig. 2
figure 2

Risk of incident dementia among the participants. Poor performance on each task of CASI was defined as performance -1.5 × the SD below the mean of the CU group. CI Confidence interval, HR Hazard ratio. Bold font indicates statistically significant

Supplementary Table 2 lists the demographic and clinical characteristics of individuals with suspected AD or vascular etiologies. Compared to individuals with suspected AD, those with vascular etiologies were younger (F 1, 1231 = 16.25, p < 0.001), better educated (F 1, 1231 = 4.25, p < 0.05), and more likely to be male (χ2 df = 1, n = 1233 = 39.38, p < 0.001). Individuals with vascular etiologies also displayed relatively poor performance on the CDR-SB during the first assessment (F 1, 1231 = 6.20, p < 0.05). Scores below the cutoff scores for the short-term memory subtest (B = 0.41, HR = 1.30, CI = 1.12 – 1.77), mental manipulation (B = 0.35, HR = 1.28, CI = 1.02 – 1.61), and orientation (B = 0.40, HR = 1.49, CI = 1.02 – 2.16) were predictive of conversion to DAT (Fig. 3 and Supplementary Fig. 2). Scores below the cutoff score for the language subtest were predictive of conversion to VaD (B = 0.46, HR = 1.58, CI = 1.07 – 2.55) (Fig. 4 and Supplementary Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Risk of incident DAT among the participants. Notes are the same as those used in Fig. 2

Fig. 4
figure 4

Risk of incident VaD among the participants. Notes are the same as those used in Fig. 2

Discussion

The study investigated the optimization of CASI cutoff scores with the aim of differentiating between individuals with or without dementia and in differentiating between individuals with MCI and those without cognitive impairment. Total CASI scores proved effective in differentiating between individuals with or without dementia across samples stratified according to demographic variables. Total CASI scores proved moderately effective in differentiating between MCI and CU individuals (lower specificity). After controlling for demographics, the CASI memory subtests were predictive of conversion from CU to dementia.

In line with previous studies [11, 22, 32], we determined that total CASI scores could be used to differentiate between individuals with or without dementia. Using the Mandarin Chinese version of CASI among Taiwanese individuals, Lin et al. [22] established optimal cutoff scores by which to differentiate between individuals with dementia and those with normal cognitive function. Note however that our cutoff scores for differentiating individuals with or without dementia were lower than those reported by Lin et al. [22] (illiterate: 49/50 vs. 47/48; low education: 67/68 vs. 55/56; high education: 79/80 vs. 72/73; younger- illiterate: 51/52 vs. 50/51; younger-low education: 67/68 vs. 60/61; younger- high education: 79/80 vs. 76/77; older-illiterate: 46/47 vs. 42/43; older-low education: 67/68 vs. 53/54; older-high education: 79/80 vs. 63/64). These differences may be attributed to our inclusion of patients with MCI in the non-dementia group, which may have enhanced the specificity of CASI scores in diagnosing dementia. Subsequent post-hoc sensitivity analysis in which patients with MCI were excluded from the non-dementia group revealed results that were very similar to those reported by Lin et al. [22] with the exception of higher cutoff scores for the illiterate groups in the current study (younger-illiterate: 58/59; older-illiterate: 50/51) (Supplementary Table 3).

Researchers have posited that MCI is a prodromal stage of dementia. MCI is generally defined as cognitive performance roughly 1.5 × SD below the normative mean in at least one cognitive domain with preservation of daily function. Teng et al. [11] suggested that sensitivity in detecting cognitive dysfunctions may differ among the various CASI tasks. They reported that short-term memory, orientation, and verbal fluency were the most effective indicators to differentiate dementia from normal cognitive function. To the best of our knowledge, no previous study has investigated the applicability of scores for CST sub-domains in detecting MCI or predicting conversion to dementia. In the current study, we determined that defining MCI as long-term memory and short-term memory scores 1.5 × the SD below the mean of the CU group was predictive of conversion to dementia. Amnestic MCI (aMCI) has been proposed as an indicator of conversion to dementia of Alzheimer’s type (DAT) [30, 33, 34]. Thus, our findings pertaining to the predictive value of CASI memory tasks may reflect a link between aMCI and related pathologically changes in the medial temporal lobe (MTL) and prefrontal cortex (PFC) and conversion to DAT [35, 36]. Note that in predicting conversion to dementia, the value of test scores for long-term memory exceeded those for short-term memory. This can perhaps be attributed to the fact that long-term memory tasks focus on long-term semantic memory retrieval (i.e., regularities and conventions), which may provide advantages in detecting early pathological changes in the MTL and PFC [37,38,39]. This may be explained by the fact that the retrieval of long-term semantic memory requires activation of the PFC [40], which interacts closely with the MTL. Interruptions to this interaction may be an early indicator of dementia.

Among the patients with incident DAT, performance on memory tasks and tasks assessing mental manipulation and orientation were also predictive of conversion to dementia. This may be due to the fact that early changes in the interactions between the MTL and PFC were predictive of conversion to DAT [37,38,39, 41]. Note that performance on long-term memory tasks was predictive only of incident all-cause dementia. These results can perhaps be attributed to the fact that the retrieval of higher-order semantic knowledge is associated with lesions in the frontosubcortical and temporal polar regions. These regions may be compromised in patients with etiologies other than AD [42,43,44].

Among patients with VaD, performance on language tasks was predictive of conversion to dementia. This is perhaps associated with vulnerabilities in the blood vessels associated with core linguistic regions, which are supported by the middle cerebral artery (MCA), the most vulnerable artery in the brain in terms of stroke [45].

In the current study, the global CASI scores proved effective in differentiating between individuals with MCI and those without cognitive impairment; however, the discriminatory power (specificity) was low. The low specificity can perhaps be attributed to the fact that the education level of many of subjects in the current study was lower than in previous studies [11, 22, 32]. This implies that the lower total CASI scores reflect a lower educational level rather than pathological changes. Taken together, it appears that specificity in MCI detection and accuracy in predicting conversion to dementia might be better served using long-term and short-term memory tasks instead of global scores.

Our findings echo those in previous studies [e.g., [9, 46, 47] indicating that education could act as a protective factor against dementia. It is possible that education enhances cognitive reserve or that a higher education and the corresponding affluence permit a healthier lifestyle (e.g., access to healthy food and time for exercise) [47, 48].

This study established optimal CASI cutoff scores to facilitate differentiation between individuals with or without dementia as well as between individuals with CU or MCI. Our findings also shed light on the predictive value of the various CASI tasks and the mechanisms underlying dementia progression. However, this study was subject to various limitations. First, the subjects were recruited at neurology clinics, such that the study group was not selected at random (selection bias). Second, it is very likely that the individuals identified as CU did not in fact retain completely normal cognitive function, considering the fact that they were seeking help from clinicians. Thus, the applicability of these cutoff scores will require further confirmation in community-wide populations. Third, the relatively short follow-up duration in this study prevented an investigation into the predictive value of CASI scores across individuals with different etiologies. Fourth, the patients were categorized using screening cognitive tests, such that some of the individuals in the CU group may actually have displayed cognitive changes that were too subtle for detection using these tests [2].

This study established optimal cutoff scores by which to determine the cognitive status of elderly individuals visiting neurology clinics. The fact that the optimal cutoff scores were lower than those reported in previous studies indicates the need to formulate population-specific cutoff scores. In the current study, performance on CASI memory tasks was found to be predictive of conversion to dementia; however, further research with longer follow-up period will be required to assess the generalizability of our findings. In the future, it may also be possible to derive short-forms of CASI that focus on memory tasks.

Availability of data and materials

Data available on request due to privacy/ethical restrictions. Requests to access these datasets should be directed to H-T C, changht@cycu.edu.tw.

References

  1. Jessen F, Amariglio RE, Buckley RF, van der Flier WM, Han Y, Molinuevo JL, Rabin L, Rentz DM, Rodriguez-Gomez O, Saykin AJ, et al. The characterisation of subjective cognitive decline. Lancet Neurol. 2020;19(3):271–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Rabin LA, Smart CM, Amariglio RE. Subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Annu Rev Clin Psycho. 2017;13:169–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Roebuck-Spencer TM, Glen T, Puente AE, Denney RL, Ruff RM, Hostetter G, Bianchini KJ. Cognitive screening tests versus comprehensive neuropsychological test batteries: A National Academy of Neuropsychology education paper. Arch Clin Neuropsych. 2017;32(4):491–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Bigler ED, Tranel D. Neuropsychological assessment. 5th ed. New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Rabin LA, Smart CM, Crane PK, Amariglio RE, Berman LM, Boada M, Buckley RF, Chételat G, Dubois B, Ellis KA, et al. Subjective cognitive decline in older adults: an overview of self-report measures used across 19 international research studies. J Alzhermers Dis. 2015;48(0 1):S63–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Jack CR Jr, Knopman DS, Jagust WJ, Petersen RC, Weiner MW, Aisen PS, Shaw LM, Vemuri P, Wiste HJ, Weigand SD, et al. Tracking pathophysiological processes in Alzheimer’s disease: an updated hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers. Lancet Neurol. 2013;12(2):207–16.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Jack CR, Bennett DA, Blennow K, Carrillo MC, Dunn B, Haeberlein SB, Holtzman DM, Jagust W, Jessen F, Karlawish J, et al. NIA-AA Research Framework: toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14(4):535–62.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Buckley RF, Saling MM, Frommann I, Wolfsgruber S. Subjective cognitive decline from a phenomenological perspective: a review of the qualitative literature. J Alzhermers Dis. 2015;48(S1):125–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Roe CM, Mintun MA, D’Angelo G, Xiong C, Grant EA, Morris JC. Alzheimer disease and cognitive reserve: variation of education effect with carbon 11-labeled Pittsburgh compound B uptake. JAMA Neurol. 2008;65(11):1467–71.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Salthouse TA. Consequences of age-related cognitive declines. Annu Rev Psychol. 2012;63(1):201–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Teng EL, Hasegawa K, Homma A, Imai Y, Larson E, Graves A, Sugimoto K, Yamaguchi T, Sasaki H, Chiu D, et al. The Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument (CASI): a practical test for cross-cultural epidemiological studies of dementia. Int Psychogeriatr. 1994;6(1):45–58.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Wang CSM, Pai MC, Chen PL, Hou NT, Chien PF, Huang YC. Montreal Cognitive Assessment and Mini-Mental State Examination performance in patients with mild-to-moderate dementia with Lewy bodies, Alzheimer’s disease, and normal participants in Taiwan. Int Psychogeriatr. 2013;25(11):1839–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Lin CM, Hung GU, Wei CY, Tzeng RC, Chiu PY. An informant-based simple questionnaire for language assessment in neurodegenerative disorders. Dement Geriatr Cogn. 2018;46(3–4):207–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Chang YF, Loi WY, Chiu P-Y, Huang H-N. Classification of dementia severity in Taiwan based on History-Based Clinical Diagnosis System. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 2020;35:1533317520970788.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Wang C-T, Hung G-U, Wei C-Y, Tzeng R-C, Chiu P-Y. An informant-based simple questionnaire for visuospatial dysfunction assessment in dementia. Front Neurosci. 2020;14:44.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Huang Y-C, Liu C-H, Liao Y-C, Chang H-T, Chiu P-Y. Arrhythmia and other modifiable risk factors in incident dementia and MCI among elderly individuals with low educational levels in Taiwan. Front Aging Neurosci. 2022;14:992532.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, Hyman BT, Jack CR Jr, Kawas CH, Klunk WE, Koroshetz WJ, Manly JJ, Mayeux R, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Ageing-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7:263–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Román GC, Tatemichi TK, Erkinjuntti T, Cummings JL, Masdeu JC, Garcia JH, Amaducci L, Orgogozo J-M, Brun A, Hofman A, et al. Vascular dementia. Diagnostic criteria for research studies: Report of the NINDS-AIREN International Workshop. Neurology. 1993;43(2):250.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. van Straaten EC, Scheltens P, Knol DL, van Buchem MA, van Dijk EJ, Hofman PA, Karas G, Kjartansson O, de Leeuw FE, Prins ND, et al. Operational definitions for the NINDS-AIREN criteria for vascular dementia: an interobserver study. Stroke. 2003;34(8):1907–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Albert MS, Dekosky ST, Dickson D, Dubois B, Fieldman HH, Snyder PJ, Carrillo MC, Thies B, Phelps CH. The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Ageing-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):270–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Dichgans M, Leys D. Vascular cognitive impairment. Circ Res. 2017;120(3):573–91.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Lin KN, Wang PN, Liu CY, Chen WT, Lee YC, Liu HC. Cutoff scores of the Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument, Chinese Version in screening of dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn. 2002;14(4):176–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Hsu J-L, Fan Y-C, Huang Y-L, Wang J, Chen W-H, Chiu H-C, Bai C-H. Improved predictive ability of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment for diagnosing dementia in a community-based study. Alzheimer’s Res Ther. 2015;7(1):69.

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  24. Tsai C-F, Lee W-J, Wang S-J, Shia B-C, Nasreddine Z, Fuh J-L. Psychometrics of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) and its subscales: validation of the Taiwanese version of the MoCA and an item response theory analysis. Int Psychogeriatr. 2011;24(4):651–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Hung C-H, Hung G-U, Wei C-Y, Tzeng R-C, Chiu P-Y. Function-based dementia severity assessment for vascular cognitive impairment. J Formos Med Assoc. 2021;120(1, Part 2):533–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. O’Bryant SE, Lacritz LH, Hall J, Waring SC, Chan W, Khodr ZG, Massman PJ, Hobson V, Cullum CM. Validation of the new interpretive guidelines for the clinical dementia rating scale sum of boxes score in the national Alzheimer’s coordinating center database. Arch Neurol. 2010;67(6):746–9.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Tzeng R-C, Yang Y-W, Hsu K-C, Chang H-T, Chiu P-Y. Sum of boxes of the clinical dementia rating scale highly predicts conversion or reversion in predementia stages. Front Aging Neurosci. 2022;14:1021792.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Winblad B, Palmer K, Kivipelto M, Jelic V, Fratiglioni L, Wahlund LO, Nordberg A, Bäckman L, Albert M, Almkvist O, et al. Mild cognitive impairment – beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Intern Med. 2004;256(3):240–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Ministry of the Interior: Statistical Yearbook of Interior. In Taipei, Taiwan; 2023. From https://ws.moi.gov.tw/001/Upload/OldFile/site_stuff/321/2/year/year_en.html.

  30. Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med. 2004;256(3):183–94.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Rulli E, Ghilotti F, Biagioli E, Porcu L, Marabese M, D’Incalci M, Bellocco R, Torri V. Assessment of proportional hazard assumption in aggregate data: a systematic review on statistical methodology in clinical trials using time-to-event endpoint. Br J Cancer. 2018;119(12):1456–63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Tsai R-C, Lin K-N, Wu K-Y, Liu H-C. Improving the screening power of the Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument. Chinese Version Dement Geriatr Cogn. 2004;18(3–4):314–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Busse A, Hensel A, Gühne U, Angermeyer MC, Rieder-Heller SG. Mild cognitive impairment: Long-term course of four subtypes. Neurology. 2006;67(12):2176–85.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Oltra-Cucarella J, Ferrer-Cascales R, Alegret M, Gasparini R, Díaz-Ortiz LM, Ríos R, Martínez-Nogueras ÁL, Onandia I, Pérez-Vicente JA, Cabello-Rodríguez L, et al. Risk of progression to Alzheimer’s disease for different neuropsychological Mild Cognitive Impairment subtypes: a hierarchical meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychol Aging. 2018;33(7):1007–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Seo SW, Im K, Lee J-M, Kim Y-H, Kim ST, Kim SY, Yang DW, Kim SI, Cho YS, Na DL. Cortical thickness in single- versus multiple-domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuroimage. 2007;36(2):289–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Clerx L, van Rossum IA, Burns L, Knol DL, Scheltens P, Verhey F, Aalten P, Lapuerta P, van de Pol L, van Schijndel R, et al. Measurements of medial temporal lobe atrophy for prediction of Alzheimer’s disease in subjects with mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol Aging. 2013;34(8):2003–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Buzsaki G, Moser EI. Memory, navigation and theta rhythm in the hippocampal-entorhinal system. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16(2):130–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Hirni DI, Kivisaari SL, Krumm S, Monsch AU, Berres M, Oeksuez F, Reinhardt J, Ulmer S, Kressiq RW, Stippich C, et al. Neuropsychological markers of medial perirhinal and entorhinal cortex functioning are impaired twelve years preceding diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzhermers Dis. 2016;52(2):573–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Woodard JL, Seidenberg M, Nielson KA, Antuono P, Guidotti L, Durgerian S, Zhang Q, Lancaster M, Hantke N, Butts A, et al. Semantic memory activation in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Brain. 2009;132(8):2068–78.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Marko M, Riečanský I. The left prefrontal cortex supports inhibitory processing during semantic memory retrieval. Cortex. 2021;134:296–306.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Ball HA, McWhirter L, Ballard C, Bhome R, Blackburn DJ, Edwards MJ, Fleming SM, Fox NC, Howard R, Huntley J, et al. Functional cognitive disorder: Dementia’s blind spot. Brain. 2020;143(10):2895–903.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Patterson K, Kopelman MD, Woollams AM, Brownsett SLE, Geranmayeh F, Wise RJS. Semantic memory: Which side are you on? Neuropsychologia. 2015;76:182–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Mesulam MM, Rogalski EJ, Wieneke C, Hurley RS, Geula C, Bigio EH, Thompson CK, Weintraub S. Primary progressive aphasia and the evolving neurology of the language network. Nat Rev Neurol. 2014;10(10):554–69.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Hart J Jr, Maguire MJ, Motes M, Mudar RA, Chiang HS, Womack KB, Kraut MA. Semantic memory retrieval circuit: role of pre-SMA, caudate, and thalamus. Brain Lang. 2013;126(1):89–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Duloquin G, Graber M, Garnier L, Crespy V, Comby P-O, Baptiste L, Mohr S, Delpont B, Guéniat J, Blanc-Labarre C, et al. Incidence of acute ischemic stroke with visible arterial occlusion. Stroke. 2020;51(7):2122–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Reuter-Lorenz PA, Park DC. How does it STAC up? Revisiting the Scaffolding Theory of Aging and Cognition. Neuropsychol Rev. 2014;24:355–70.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, Brayne C, Burns A, Cohen-Mansfield J, Cooper C, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet. 2020;396(10248):413–46.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Sun Y, Lee H-J, Yang S-C, Chen T-F, Lin K-N, Lin C-C, Wang P-N, Tang L-Y, Chiu M-J. A nationwide survey of mild cognitive impairment and dementia, including very mild dementia, in Taiwan. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(6):e100303.

    Article  ADS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the members of HAICDDS for their help in completing this research.

Funding

This study was not supported by any funding.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis and investigation, writing (original draft preparation): WCL, PYC. Writing (review and editing), supervision: CHL. Writing (review and editing): YCL. Conceptualization, methodology, writing (review and editing), supervision: HTC.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hsin-Te Chang.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Show Chwan Memorial Hospital (SCMH_IRB1081006) and Informed consent was waived by the Institutional Review Board of the Show Chwan Memorial Hospital (SCMH_IRB1081006) because of the retrospective nature of the study design and data were processed anonymously.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests..

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lyu, WJ., Chiu, PY., Liu, CH. et al. Determining optimal cutoff scores of Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument to identify dementia and mild cognitive impairment in Taiwan. BMC Geriatr 24, 216 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-024-04810-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-024-04810-y

Keywords