Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Association between cognitive function and dusty weather: a propensity score matching study

Abstract

Background

With a rapidly aging global population, the health of older adults is a national priority for countries across the world. Dusty weather has been demonstrated to be a potential risk factor of cognitive function among the elderly population. However, there is a paucity of studies exploring the associations between dusty weather and cognitive function among the older in China.

Methods

Data on individual characteristics were obtained from the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Survey (CHARLS) 2018, whereas data on air pollution were sourced from environmental monitoring stations in China. Cognitive function, including general cognitive function, episodic memory, and linguistic competence, was assessed by self- or informant-questionnaires. We used propensity score matching and linear regression to investigate the relationship between dusty weather and cognitive function. Sensitivity analyses were conducted to test the robustness of the results.

Results

This study included 8,604 participants older than 60 years old. After controlling air pollutant weather, dusty weather was demonstrated to be positively associated with a decline in cognitive function (Informant Questionnaire on Cognitive Decline in the Elderly (IQCODE), 4.0, 95% confidence interval (CI): 3.11, 4.89; Miniā€Mental State Exam (MMSE), 0.63, 95% CI: 0.34, 0.92). Results of sensitivity analysis showed that our research findings are robust.

Conclusion

Older adults living in dusty weather regions suffered a higher level of cognitive impairment, and such adverse effects were more substantial among females compared with their male counterparts. Targeted health interventions to help older adults living in regions where dusty weather occurs frequently are suggested to be proposed.

Peer Review reports

Background

Cognitive function encompasses the performance of the mental action or processes of perception, learning, memory, understanding, awareness, reasoning, judgment, intuition, and language [1]. It is often measured through parameters like attention span, learning ability, problem-solving, and more [2]. Recent findings indicate that diminished mental processing speed, working memory deficits, and attention inhibition are primary contributors to age-related cognitive decline in older adults [3]. As the world's older population is expanding at an unprecedented rate [4], it becomes crucial to identify factors impacting cognitive function in this demographic.

The impact of age on cognitive function in older adults is multifaceted, with both genetic and environmental factors playing significant roles. Specific inflammatory genes like rs16944 have been shown to

be associated with a generalized cognitive decline, whereas others, such as IL1Ī² SNP rs16944, have been connected solely to memory loss in older adults [5]. Environmental factors, particularly those related to the natural environment, also influence cognitive function through complex mechanisms. For instance, previous studies have shown that road traffic noise [6], distance to natural outdoor environments from a residential address [7], and green space [8] were factors affecting cognitive function in the older. Additionally, This body of research shows that air pollutants such as particulate matter (PM), sulfur dioxide (SO2), and carbon monoxide (CO) can directly or indirectly lead to cognitive impairment [9,10,11,12]. However, much of this research concerns chronic exposure, leaving a knowledge gap regarding the effects of transient increases in air pollutants specifically caused by dusty weather.

Dusty weather is a prevalent meteorological condition in regions like Northern Africa, Central Asia, Australia, and China, encompassing blowing dust, dust haze, dust devils, and Haboobs. It is associated with elevated PM, SO2, and CO levels, leading to potential restrictions on outdoor activities [13, 14] and sunlight exposure [15, 16]. Thus, it could be a potential risk factor for cognitive decline, not least because of the inflammatory mechanisms and white matter injury caused by these pollutants [17].

While the effects of long-term air pollutant exposure on cognitive decline have been extensively studied, the influence of transient increases in air pollutants caused by dusty weather remains relatively unexplored [10, 13]. Also, there is a notable lack of studies exploring the impacts of dusty weather on cognitive function in older adults. Most previous research focused on the association between dusty weather and circulatory diseases, respiratory diseases, eye symptoms, cognitive development in teenagers [18, 19], suicide [20], and suicide rates. Moreover, a crucial aspect that remains overlooked in the most existing literature is the need to control air pollution factors potentially caused by dusty weather [18].

In response to these gaps in the existing literature, this study aims to explore the association between dusty weather and cognitive function in older adults, controlling for the effects of air pollutants. The propensity score matching and linear regression were adopted to analyze the impacts of dusty weather on cognitive function the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Survey 2018 (CHARLS 2018). The study followed the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) Statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies [21].

Method

Data source

Individual characteristic data were extracted from Wave 4 of CHARLS. This wave was chosen because it concentrated on the various dimensions of cognitive function in older populations. CHARLS is a national longitudinal survey launched in 2012. It aims to establish a high-quality public database for individuals aged 45 or older in China [22]. The survey encompasses over 17,000 participants from 450 communities at 150 county-level units (including prefecture-level cities or county-level cities) across 28 provinces of China [22]. The data was collected using a four-stage stratified random cluster sampling method. CHARLS Wave 4 was conducted in 2018, collecting information from 19,816 participants with a response rate of 83.84%. After excluding individuals younger than 60 and samples with missing data, 8604 participants (4032 exposed and 4032 non-exposed) were included in our analysis.

Data for the air quality index (AQI), PM2.5, PM10, SO2, nitrogen dioxide (NO2), ozone (O3), and CO were retrieved from the China National Environmental Monitor Center [23]. The daily average value for each air pollutant in each city was estimated using data from the monitoring station in that city.

Variables

The study participants were classified into exposure and non-exposure groups based on the frequency of dusty weather events. Frequently, dusty weather occurs in arid and semi-arid regions of central China [24]. As per environmental reports released by the local meteorological administration, participants residing in China, where dusty weather occurred more than three times yearly from 2015 to 2017, were included in the exposure group.

In the older population, cognitive function, including general cognitive function, episodic memory, linguistic competence, delayed recall, and informant reports of cognitive function, was assessed using a range of tests. The general cognitive function was evaluated using the Miniā€Mental State Exam (MMSE) [25] and Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status (TICS) [26], separately. In the MMSE, a score of 25 or higher is deemed normal, with scores below 24 usually indicative of potential cognitive impairment [25]. In the TICS, the normative reference group for interpretation varies based on the purpose of the evaluation and the examinee's age and education level [26]. Episodic memory was evaluated through a recall test involving a list of words presented to the participants, and linguistic competence was measured using the animal fluency test (AFT) - a test quantifying the number of distinct animals a participant could name within a minute [27]. A delayed recall test was performed to measure the delayed recall memory by evaluating the ability to recall specific information after a period of rest or distraction from that information. Informant reports of cognitive function were measured by the Informant Questionnaire on Cognitive Decline in the Elderly (IQCODE) and Community Screening Instrument-Dementia (CSI-D) part 2. A more detailed explanation of these scales and their administration procedures can be found in Supplementary Material 1.

A series of covariates were also considered. These included sociodemographic characteristics, such as age, sex, marital status, minority status, and residential address type; socioeconomic characteristics like education level and location; behavioral factors, such as smoking and drinking; and pre-existing health conditions, including stroke, Alzheimer's disease, Parkinson's disease, and memory problems.

Statistical analyses

We used multiple imputations by predictive mean matching to impute missing in our primary analysis. Propensity score matching (PSM) was performed using a 1:1 nearest neighbor matching algorithm without replacement with distances determined by logistic regression. Propensity score matching was performed based on the following variables: age, sex, education, marital status, minority, type of residential address, locations, smoking, drinking, and pre-existing health conditions (stroke, Alzheimerā€™s disease, Parkinsonā€™s disease, and memory problems). The distribution of demographic information before and after propensity score matching is shown in Table 1 and Supplementary Table 1. Comparisons of participant characteristics in the unmatched and matched cohorts, with and without exposure to dusty weather, were performed using the Mann-Whitney U test for continuous variables and the chi-square test for categorical variables. After performing the propensity score, the absolute standardized mean difference in distances between individuals in the exposure and non-exposure group was balanced, with differences less than 0.1 indicative of a good balance. Considering the correlation within cities in the same province, we utilized a linear regression model with clustered standard errors to estimate the association between dusty weather and cognitive function. Additional covariates, such as the city's gross development product in 2018, AQI, PM2.5, PM10, SO2, NO2, O3, and CO, were incorporated into the adjusted regression model. A subgroup analysis was also conducted by gender to account for potential differences in covariates between groups. To verify the robustness of our results, we conducted two sensitivity analyses. The first analysis was based on the pre-matching data, while the second incorporated varying covariates into the propensity score calculation. The estimated effect sizes and 95% Confidence Intervals (CIs) are presented in the results section. All statistical analyses were executed using R programming version 4.2.3.

Table 1 Descriptive characteristics of sampled older adults after PSM

Results

Descriptive results

After eliminating 9075 samples of individuals under 60 and 1137 unmatched samples, the study was conducted with exposure and non-exposure samples comprising 4302 and 4302 individuals, respectively. The median age in the exposure group was 68 years (interquartile range: 64 - 74 years) compared to 68 years in the non-exposure group (interquartile range: 64 - 74 years). Females constituted 50.1% of the exposure group and 46.4% of the non-exposure group (Table 1). Most participants were of Han minority origin (93.1% exposure; 93.0% non-exposure), resided in family homes (98.2% exposure; 98.2% non-exposure), and lived in rural areas (74.8% exposure; 73.4% non-exposure).

Supplementary Tables 2 and 3 provide a comparative assessment of air pollutants and cognitive function by exposure status. The concentration of all pollutants except O3 in the central dust storm regions was significantly higher than that in other areas. Cognitive function scoresā€”including self-reported general cognitive function, episodic memory, linguistic competence, and informant-reported cognitive functionā€”were restricted growth in the central dust storm regions.

Association between dusty weather and cognitive function

Figure 1 reports the relationship between dusty weather and cognitive function in older adults. Our findings indicated a positive correlation between dusty weather and various cognitive tests: episodic memory (wordlist recall: 0.50; 95% CI: 0.30, 0.70), general cognitive function (MMSE: 0.63; 95%, 0.34, 0.92), and informant-reported cognitive function (CSI2: 0.49; 95% CI: 0.40, 0.58). Despite the positive impact of dusty weather on delayed recall (delayed recall test: 0.09; 95% CI: 0.00, 0.19), this correlation was not statistically significant.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Unadjusted and adjusted associations between dusty weather and cognitive function in the general population

After adjusting for air pollution, we found a statistically significant but weaker correlation between dusty weather and cognitive function, except for the delayed recall test (delayed recall tes: 0.10; 95% CI: -0.03, 0.23).

Subgroup analysis and sensitivity analysis

A subgroup analysis was performed with respect to gender, as depicted in Figs.Ā 2 and 3. The data show a significant correlation between dusty weather and cognitive functions in older male adults, but this significance was only observed in models that did not adjust for air pollutants. When these adjustments were made, the associations between most cognitive function indicators (such as the Mini-Mental State Examination and delayed recall) and dusty weather exposure became statistically insignificant.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Unadjusted and adjusted associations between dusty weather and cognitive function in males

Fig. 3
figure 3

Unadjusted and Adjusted associations between dusty weather and cognitive function in the females

In contrast, the relationship between dusty weather and cognitive functions in older female adults remained statistically significant in models with and without adjustments for air pollutants, except for the delayed recall test. Interestingly, the impacts of dusty weather on most of the four cognitive function measures were more pronounced in older female adults than in the total sample or male-only group.

The results of the sensitivity analysis can be found in Supplementary Table 4. The findings from both sensitivity analyses align with the primary results, suggesting robustness in our study outcomes.

Discussion

After controlling for air pollutants, this study revealed that dusty weather is associated with cognitive function in old persons. The positive impacts of dusty weather on cognitive function in older female adults were found to be larger than those in the whole sample or males.

Our study confirmed that dusty weather resulted in cognitive impairment in the older through increased concentrations of air pollutants. Previous research has shown that dusty weather increases the concentration of air pollutants [17], which in turn leads to age-related cognitive impairment [28]. Another potential mechanism is that dusty weather has negative impacts on outdoor activities, which can further lead to cognitive impairment [16]. Besides, dusty weather was also positively associated with an increased risk of cardiovascular diseases that can lead to cognitive impairment [29].

This study also demonstrates the adverse effects of dusty weather on cognitive function were more substantial in females and older adults, which is consistent with a number of previous studies [30, 31]. For instance, a Korean study demonstrated that the relationship between NO2 and cognitive function was stronger in females [30]. A study conducted in Mexico has also shown that the positive effects of PM2.5 on cognitive function in girls were larger than those in boys [31]. This finding may be explained by aĀ stronger female-specificĀ impactĀ of theĀ APOE-Īµ4, a major genetic risk factor, on age-related cognitive decline [32]. Another possible explanation is that dusty weather or air pollutants can particularly lead to some women-specific diseases like hypertensive pregnancy disorders [33] and menopause [34] that have been reported as risk factors of cognitive decline.

This study suggests further promoting the ā€œGreen Great Wallā€ (GGW), a project established by the Chinese government to build vegetation protective barriers throughout China from the southwestern to the northeastern. A recent study found that during dusty weather in northern China, GGW significantly reduced the concentration of air pollutants, potentially reducing the adverse effects of dusty weather on cognitive function [35].

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study that explored the potential relationship between dusty weather and cognitive function in older adults in China. This study's evidence could serve as a guide for designing future cognitive intervention programs and an essential reference for other countries facing severe dusty weather issues. Moreover, we controlled the previous diseases that probably influenced the cognitive function in this analysis, increasing our results' validity. Some limitations of this study should be recognized. First, air pollutants were measured at the city level, which may impair the precision of our estimation results. Future studies with individual-level air pollution data can generate more accurate results. Second, limited by data availability, our analysis fails to control other dusty weather-related air pollutants. These air pollutants may exaggerate the relationship between dusty weather and cognitive function. Also, although the correlation between cognitive function and dusty weather was still robust after adjusting for various air pollutants, it is unknown whether our research findings remain consistent after controlling for unobserved additional confounders or mediators. Besides, based on the sampling method, there is probably some oversampling of participants from rural areas and from some provinces in CHARLS. But, because of the lack of individual distribution information in those cities, we can not apply sampling weighting, which may cause potential selection bias. We highly recommend further studies to explore the underlying mechanisms of association. There is abundant room for future research to include more confounding factors to estimate the causal association between dusty weather and cognitive function.

Conclusions

This study illustrated positive associations between dusty weather and linguistic competence and general cognitive function even after controlling air pollutants. What we found proved the effectiveness of GGW policy from a public health perspective. More interventions should be done to reduce the damage of dusty weather on individuals who live in dusty weather areas.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are available in the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Survey (http://charls.pku.edu.cn/index/en.html) and China National Environmental Monitoring Center (http://www.cnemc.cn/en/).

Abbreviations

AFT:

Animal Fluency Test

AQI:

Air quality index

CO:

Carbon monoxide

CHARLS 2018:

China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Survey 2018

CSI-D:

Community Screening Instrument-Dementia

GGW:

Green Great Wall

IQCODE:

Informant Questionnaire on Cognitive Decline in the Elderly

MMSE:

Miniā€Mental State Exam

NO2 :

Nitrogen dioxide

O3 :

Ozone

PM:

Particulate matter

PSM:

Propensity score matching

SO2 :

Sulfur dioxide

TICS:

Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status

References

  1. Association AP. APA dictionary of psychology 2020. Available from: https://dictionary.apa.org/just-world-hypothesis.

  2. McDougall GJ. A review of screening instruments for assessing cognition and mental status in older adults. Nurse Pract. 1990;15(11):18ā€“28.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  3. Cognitive aging: A primer. Park DC, Schwarz N, editors. New York: Psychology Press; 2000. xiii, 292-xiii.

  4. Leeson GW. The growth, ageing and urbanisation of our world. J Popula Ageing. 2018;11(2):107ā€“15.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  5. Stacey D, Ciobanu LG, Baune BT. A systematic review on the association between inflammatory genes and cognitive decline in non-demented elderly individuals. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2017;27(6):568ā€“88.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  6. Fuks KB, Wigmann C, Altug H, Schikowski T. Road Traffic Noise at the Residence, Annoyance, and Cognitive Function in Elderly Women. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019;16:1790. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16101790.

  7. Zijlema WL, Triguero-Mas M, Smith G, Cirach M, Martinez D, Dadvand P, et al. The relationship between natural outdoor environments and cognitive functioning and its mediators. Environ Res. 2017;155:268ā€“75.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  8. Cherrie MPC, Shortt NK, Mitchell RJ, Taylor AM, Redmond P, Thompson CW, et al. Green space and cognitive ageing: a retrospective life course analysis in the Lothian Birth Cohort 1936. Soc Sci Med. 2018;196:56ā€“65.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  9. Schikowski T, Altuğ H. The role of air pollution in cognitive impairment and decline. Neurochemi Int. 2020;136:104708.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  10. World Health Organization. Regional Office for E. Review of evidence on health aspects of air pollution: REVIHAAP project: technical report. Copenhagen: World Health Organization; 2021. Regional Office for Europe; 2021 2021. Contract No.: WHO/EURO:2013-2663-42419-58845.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  11. Zare Sakhvidi MJ, Yang J, Lequy E, Chen J, de Hoogh K, Letellier N, et al. Outdoor air pollution exposure and cognitive performance: findings from the enrolment phase of the CONSTANCES cohort. Lancet Planet Health. 2022;6(3):e219ā€“29.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  12. Zhang X, Chen X, Zhang X. The impact of exposure to air pollution on cognitive performance. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2018;115(37):9193ā€“7.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  13. Tao M, Chen L, Wang Z, Ma P, Tao J, Jia S. A study of urban pollution and haze clouds over northern China during the dusty season based on satellite and surface observations. Atmos Environ. 2014;82:183ā€“92.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  14. Rezaei S, Naddafi K, Hassanvand MS, Nabizadeh R, Yunesian M, Ghanbarian M, et al. Physiochemical characteristics and oxidative potential of ambient air particulate matter (PM10) during dust and non-dust storm events: a case study in Tehran Iran. J Environ Health Sci Eng. 2018;16(2):147ā€“58.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  15. Ren L, Zheng Y, Wu L, Gu Y, He Y, Jiang B, et al. Investigation of the prevalence of Cognitive Impairment and its risk factors within the elderly population in Shanghai, China. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):3575.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  16. Gao Q, Luan D, Wang X, Xin S, Liu Y, Li J. Effect of sun exposure on cognitive function among elderly individuals in Northeast China. Clin Interv Aging. 2018;13:2075ā€“82.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  17. Babadjouni RM, Hodis DM, Radwanski R, Durazo R, Patel A, Liu Q, et al. Clinical effects of air pollution on the central nervous system; a review. J ClinNeurosci. 2017;43:16ā€“24.

    CASĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  18. LĆ³pez-Cayuela M, CĆ³rdoba-Jabonero C, Bermejo-PantaleĆ³n D, Sicard M, Salgueiro V, Molero F, et al. Vertical characterization of fine and coarse dust particles during an intense Saharan dust outbreak over the Iberian Peninsula in springtime 2021.Ā Atmos Chem Phys. 2023;23(1):143ā€“61. https://doi.org/10.5194/acp-23-143-2023.

  19. Aghababaeian H, Ostadtaghizadeh A, Ardalan A, Asgary A, Akbary M, Yekaninejad MS, et al. Global health impacts of dust storms: a systematic review. Environ Health Insights. 2021;15:11786302211018390.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  20. Lee H, Jung J, Myung W, Baek JH, Kang JM, Kim DK, et al. Association between dust storm occurrence and risk of suicide: Case-crossover analysis of the Korean national death database. Environ Int. 2019;133:105146.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  21. Vandenbroucke JP, von Elm E, Altman DG, GĆøtzsche PC, Mulrow CD, Pocock SJ, et al. Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE): Explanation and Elaboration. PLOS Med. 2007;4(10):e297.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  22. Zhao Y, Hu Y, Smith JP, Strauss J, Yang G. Cohort Profile: The China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS). Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43(1):61ā€“8.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  23. Centre CNEM. 2021. Available from: http://www.cnemc.cn/.

  24. Wang X, Dong Z, Zhang J, Liu L. Modern dust storms in China: an overview. J Arid Environ. 2004;58(4):559ā€“74.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  25. Mitrushina M, Satz P. Reliability and validity of the mini-mental state exam in neurologically intact elderly. J Clin Psychol. 1991;47(4):537ā€“43.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  26. Seo EH, Lee DY, Kim SG, Kim KW, Kim DH, Kim Bj, et al. Validity of the telephone interview for cognitive status (TICS) and modified TICS (TICSm) for mild cognitive imparment (MCI) and dementia screening. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2011;52(1):e26ā€“30.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  27. Campagna F, Montagnese S, Ridola L, Senzolo M, Schiff S, De Rui M, et al. The animal naming test: an easy tool for the assessment of hepatic encephalopathy. Hepatology. 2017;66(1):198ā€“208.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  28. Cipriani G, Danti S, Carlesi C, Borin G. Danger in the Air: Air Pollution and Cognitive Dysfunction. Am J Alzheimerā€™s Dis Other DementĀ®. 2018;33(6):333ā€“41.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  29. Stefanidis KB, Askew CD, Greaves K, Summers MJ. The effect of non-stroke cardiovascular disease states on risk for cognitive decline and dementia: a systematic and meta-analytic review. Neuropsychol Rev. 2018;28(1):1ā€“15.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  30. Kim H, Noh J, Noh Y, Oh SS, Koh SB, Kim C. Gender Difference in the Effects of Outdoor Air Pollution on Cognitive Function Among Elderly in Korea. Front Public Health. 2019;7:375.

  31. CalderĆ³n-GarcidueƱas L, Jewells V, Galaz-Montoya C, van Zundert B, PĆ©rez-Calatayud A, Ascencio-Ferrel E, et al. Interactive and additive influences of Gender, BMI and Apolipoprotein 4 on cognition in children chronically exposed to high concentrations of PM2.5 and ozone. APOE 4 females are at highest risk in Mexico City. Environ Res. 2016;150:411ā€“22.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  32. Nebel RA, Aggarwal NT, Barnes LL, Gallagher A, Goldstein JM, Kantarci K, et al. Understanding the impact of sex and gender in Alzheimerā€™s disease: a call to action. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14(9):1171ā€“83.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  33. Mielke MM, Milic NM, Weissgerber TL, White WM, Kantarci K, Mosley TH, et al. Impaired cognition and brain atrophy decades after hypertensive pregnancy disorders. Circ Cardiovasc Qual Outcomes. 2016;9(2 Suppl 1):S70-6.

    PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  34. Rocca WA, Grossardt BR, Shuster LT. Oophorectomy, menopause, estrogen treatment, and cognitive aging: clinical evidence for a window of opportunity. Brain Res. 2011;1379:188ā€“98.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  35. Long X, Tie X, Li G, Cao J, Feng T, Zhao S, et al. Effect of ecological restoration programs on dust concentrations in the North China Plain: a case study. Atmos Chem Phys. 2018;18(9):6353ā€“66.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

Natural Science Foundation of Hunan Province. The funding bodies played no role in the design of the study and collection, analysis, and interpretation of data and in writing the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

The corresponding author XS contributed to the study conception and data collection. and HY performed material preparation, data analysis, and academic writing. ZP took part in the manuscript revision.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Xinping Sha.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable. The manuscript does not report on or involve the use of any animal or human tissue, and all data are from the dataset and kept anonymous. Changde Xiangya Hospital ethics committee waived this ethics approval because this is unnecessary based on regulations.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1:

Ā Supplementary Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of sampled older adults before PSM. Supplementary Table 2. The comparison of air pollutants in exposure and non-exposure regions. Supplementary Table 3. The comparison of cognitive function in exposure and non-exposure groups. Supplementary Table 4. Sensitivity analysis ā€“ the relationship between dusty weather and cognitive impairment.

Additional file 2.

Ā 

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Yao, H., Peng, Z. & Sha, X. Association between cognitive function and dusty weather: a propensity score matching study. BMC Geriatr 23, 777 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-023-04466-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-023-04466-0

Keywords