Skip to main content

Napping and cognitive decline: a systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies

Abstract

Background

No clear evidence is available for the influence of napping on cognitive function in older adults. This systematic review and meta-analysis aimed to elucidate the cross-sectional and longitudinal relationships between napping and cognitive function (global cognition and memory) and to explore whether some individual characteristics and sleep characteristics can modify this relationship.

Methods

We systematically searched Medline (via PubMed), Web of Science, and Scopus. DerSimonian and Lair and Hartung-Knapp-Sidik-Jonkman random effects methods were used to compute pooled estimates of odds ratios (ORs) and their respective 95% confidence intervals (95% CIs) for the association of global cognition and memory with napping. The mean age, the night sleep time (hours), and the percentage of women, no nappers, and people in the less night-time sleep duration category were used for meta-regressions.

Results

Twenty-five studies were included in this systematic review and meta-analysis, 18 cross-sectional and seven longitudinal studies, including 95,719 participants older than 60 years. The pooled ORs from the cross-sectional analyses were 1.03 (95% CI: 1.01 to 1.06) for global cognition and 1.06 (95%: 0.90 to 1.26) for memory. The pooled ORs from the longitudinal analyses were 1.00 (95% 0.85 to 1.18) for global cognition and 1.08 (95% 0.98 to 1.19) for memory. These associations were not modified by individual or sleep characteristics.

Conclusion

Our data confirm the absence of association between napping and global cognition and memory regardless of the characteristics of the population. This information might be considered when providing lifestyle recommendations to adults with and without cognitive complaints.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Aging is accompanied by several physical, social, and psychological changes [1]. These changes may lead to chronic diseases such as dementia and Alzheimer’s disease, the estimated costs and prevalence of which have been increasing in recent years [2]. Due to the limited effects demonstrated by pharmacological treatments, new approaches are needed to address the increasing incidence of cognitive decline and dementia diseases [3]. Thus, nonpharmacological preventive and therapeutic strategies have become key tools to manage cognitive decline and dementia due to their feasibility and safety [4, 5].

Among nonpharmacological treatments, the management of health parameters described as risk factors for the development of dementia has been considered an important element [6]. Additionally, some behaviors, including but not limited to sleep patterns, have been reported to potentially be related to cognitive function [7]. Although changes in the structure and organization of sleep time increase with aging and with the incidence of chronic diseases [8, 9], the direction of the relationship between sleep disorders and cognitive decline is still not clear.

A nap is defined as a short sleep episode typically during daylight hours, the duration of which can range from a few minutes to several hours, while the frequency can vary from occasional naps to several naps daily [10]. Although nap characteristics such as the duration, frequency, intention, and depth of napping might be considered, [11, 12] positive effects of napping have been reported for physical health, cognitive function, and mood [8]. Thus, napping for enjoyment or replacement reasons has been established as an approach to maintain physical activity levels and improve performance [13, 14]. Moreover, excessive daytime napping might be associated with sedentary behaviors and depletion of leisure time activities [15], which may in turn negatively impact cognitive function [8], although not all cognitive domains are equally affected by napping [15]. Additionally, some individual characteristics, including sex [16] and age [17], may influence the relationship between napping and cognitive function.

As a previous narrative review claims, more research is urgently needed to investigate the influence of napping on health in older adults [18]. Therefore, this systematic review and meta-analysis was aimed to quantify the cross-sectional and longitudinal relationships between napping and cognitive function among the general population, distinguishing between global cognition and memory, and to explore whether some individual and sleep characteristics can modify this relationship.

Methods

We conducted this systematic review and meta-analysis following the PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews (Supplementary Table 1) [19] and the Cochrane Collaboration Handbook [20]. The protocol for this systematic review and meta-analysis has been previously registered on PROSPERO: CRD42021232071.

Data sources and searches

A literature search was performed in Medline (via PubMed), Web of Science, and Scopus to identify studies on the association between napping and cognitive function among the adult general population through August 6, 2022. The search strategy combined the following terms: “napping”, “siesta”, “nap”, “nap sleep”, “nap time”, “day sleep”, “daytime sleep”, “daytime nap”, “daytime napping”, “day time sleep”, “day time nap”, “day time napping”, “day-time sleep”, “day-time nap”, “day-time napping”, “elderly”, “older adults”, “older adult”, “middle-aged adults”, “aged individuals”, “aged adults”, senior*, “ancient”, “aging”, “cognition,” “executive,” “executive function,” “cognitive control,” “memory,” “attention,” “metacognition,” “life skills,” “goal setting,” “problem solving,” “self-regulation,” “brain development,” “brain health,” and “neural” (Supplementary Tables 2). We completed the literature search by reviewing the reference lists of the included studies for any further relevant study.

Study selection

This systematic review includes cross-sectional and longitudinal studies on the relationship between napping and cognitive function among adults. The inclusion criteria were as follows: (1) participants: general population which mean age was older than 60; (2) exposure: napping; and (3) outcome: cognitive function measured using standardized tests.

Studies were excluded when they were (1) focused on children or adolescents, (2) focused on specific populations such as people with dementia or Parkinson’s disease, (3) focused on how progressive cognitive decline could influence daily sleep duration and frequency, or (4) written in languages other than English, French, Portuguese, or Spanish.

The cognitive functions most consistently reported were i) global cognition using the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA), Trail Making Test (TMT) -A or B, figure drawing, and clock drawing tests; and ii) memory using the Logical Memory II (LM-II), Controlled Oral Word Associated Test (COWAT), and word recall tests. Other cognitive functions measured included inhibition, executive functions, psychomotor speed, self-reported cognitive difficulties, and visuospatial reasoning.

Data extraction and quality assessment

The main characteristics of the included studies are summarized in tables, including information on (1) subject characteristics (i.e., sample size, the percentage of women, the mean age, and depressive symptoms), (2) exposure (i.e., the device used to measure napping, the total night-time sleep duration, and the total napping time or frequency as reported by original studies), and (3) outcome information (i.e., tests used to measure cognitive function and cognitive domains). Covariates included in the analyses reported by the included studies were summarized in an additional table.

The Quality Assessment Tool for Observational Cohort and Cross-Sectional Studies [21] was used to evaluate the risk of bias. This tool evaluates 14 criteria for longitudinal studies; for cross-sectional designs only 11 were applied. Each criterion can be scored as “yes” when the study achieves the criterion or “no” when the study does not achieve the criterion. Criteria could also be scored as “not reported” when studies did not clearly report the required information.

The literature search, data extraction, and risk of bias assessment were independently performed by 2 researchers (C.A.-B. and A.E.-M.), and disagreements were resolved by consensus or involving a third researcher (V.M.-V.).

Data synthesis and statistical analysis

To perform the meta-analysis, measures of the association between napping and cognitive function were included in the analysis. We considered only two domains for the statistical analysis, namely, global cognition and memory, for which cross-sectional and longitudinal association analyses were separately conducted. These two domains were the ones most consistently reported across studies, and both were managed for this meta-analysis as reported by the original studies. Meta-analyses or graphical representation for other domains could not be conducted as not enough data were available.

Both the DerSimonian [22] and Lair and Hartung-Knapp-Sidik-Jonkman [23] random effects methods were used to compute pooled estimates of odds ratios (ORs) and their respective 95% confidence intervals (95% CIs) for the association of global cognition and memory with napping. Inconsistency across studies [24, 25] was assessed using the I2 statistic, whose values were considered as follows: not important (0–40%), moderate (30–60%), substantial (50–90%), and considerable (75–100%). Moreover, the corresponding P values were also considered [22]. In addition, heterogeneity [26] was evaluated by the τ2 statistic, which was interpreted as low when τ2 was lower than 0.04, moderate when τ2 ranged from 0.04 to 0.14, and as substantial when τ2 ranged from 0.14 to 0.40 [27].

When studies provided ≥ 2 measurements for the same cognitive domain using different tests (e.g., immediate and delayed word recall for memory), these measurements were combined to calculate a single pooled OR for the corresponding domain. For the analyses, we considered the data adjusted by the largest number of covariates. When regression models were presented, only those using “no nappers” as reference were considered for the analyses. When studies reported associations by group, data were included as different cohorts in the analyses. Finally, when studies provided a linear regression b coefficient, it was used to calculate OR values [28].

Sensitivity analyses were performed excluding studies one by one from the pooled estimates to evaluate whether any particular study modified the original summary estimate. Meta-regressions were calculated on the basis of sample characteristics: the mean age, the night sleep time (hours), and the percentage of women, no nappers, and people in the less night-time sleep duration category. Finally, small study effects were estimated using Egger’s test [29].

Results

Systematic review

The literature search retrieved 982 studies, 25 of which were included in this systematic review and meta-analysis [30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54] (Supplementary Fig. 1). The cross-sectional analysis included 18 studies published from 1996 to 2022, reporting data from 82,757 participants older than 60 years. Furthermore, the seven longitudinal studies reported data for 12,962 participants older than 64 years. Each longitudinal study reported a different follow-up period ranging from 6 months to 11 years. The population reported a total sleep duration ranging from less than 5 h to more than 9 h. The characteristics of napping were diversely reported by the studies, including i) the napping time, ii) the percentage of nappers, iii) the frequency of napping in days per week, and iv) the intentionality (intentional or unintentional) of napping (Supplementary Tables 3 and 4).

Finally, a different set of covariates was used to adjust the analyses reported by the included studies (Supplementary Table 5).

Risk of bias

After assessing the risk of bias by the Quality Assessment Tool for Observational Cohort and Cross-Sectional Studies, cross-sectional studies met 7 to 8 criteria, and longitudinal studies met 9 to 11 criteria. No study reported information on the sample size calculation or for the blinded assessment of participants. Moreover, four cross-sectional studies reported a participation rate of eligible persons lower than 50%, and three longitudinal studies presented a loss of follow-up after baseline higher than 20% (Supplementary Table 6).

Meta-analysis

Using the DerSimoniand and Lair random effect models, the pooled ORs from the cross-sectional analyses were 1.03 (95% CI: 1.01 to 1.06) for global cognition and 1.06 (95%: 0.90 to 1.26) for memory. The pooled ORs from the longitudinal analyses were 1.00 (95% 0.85 to 1.18) for global cognition and 1.08 (95% 0.98 to 1.19) for memory (Figs. 1 and 2). Similar results were obtained using the Hartung-Knapp-Sidik-Jonkman random effect models, the pooled ORs from the cross-sectional analyses were 1.10 (95% CI: 0.99 to 1.20) for global cognition and 1.08 (95%: 0.81 to 1.34) for memory. The pooled ORs from the longitudinal analyses were 0.94 (95% 0.73 to 1.15) for global cognition and 1.07 (95% 0.96 to 1.18) for memory (Supplementary Table 7).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Forest plot for the cross-sectional association between napping and cognitive function domains. ES indicates effect size

Fig. 2
figure 2

Forest plot for the longitudinal association between napping and cognitive function domains. ES indicates effect size

Data for inconsistency and heterogeneity for both models are presented in Supplementary Table 7.

Meta-regression and sensitivity analysis

Meta-regression analyses indicated that none of the considered characteristics (i.e., the mean age, the night sleep time (hours), and the percentage of women, no nappers, and people in the less nighttime sleep duration category) influence the cross-sectional or longitudinal relationships between napping with global cognition and memory (Supplementary Tables 8 and 9).

Sensitivity analysis revealed that the pooled ORs were modified after removing from the: i) cross-sectional analysis of napping and global cognition, the men and women cohorts included in Chiu et al. study; and ii) cross-sectional analysis of napping and memory and the longitudinal analysis of napping and memory, the women cohort of Sha et al. study (Supplementary Tables 10 and 11).

Small study effects

Small study effects was observed for the cross-sectional analysis of the association between napping and global cognition (Supplementary Table 12).

Discussion

This study aimed to assess the cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between napping and cognitive function among older adults. Our data suggest no association between napping and global cognition or memory. Additionally, these findings were not modified by individual characteristics, including the mean age, the night sleep time (hours), and the percentage of women, no nappers, and people in the less nighttime sleep duration category.

Napping is a well-established sedentary behavior in many countries and a more common behavior as individuals become older. Therefore, this behavior has been traditionally studied to elucidate its positive and negative effects on health [9]. Although previous research has suggested both positive and negative associations of napping with cognitive function [37, 43, 46,47,48,49,50, 52,53,54], our data indicate no cross-sectional association between napping and specific cognitive functions, including global cognition and memory. Additionally, a U-shape association has been suggested to describe the cross-sectional relationship between napping and cognitive function [33, 53], which we cannot confirm because the duration and frequency of napping were not reported by all the included studies.

The longitudinal association between napping and cognitive functions has been considered a two-way relationship. While a negative effect has been reported for napping on cognitive functions, patients with cognitive decline have also been reported to tend to sleep more during the day as cognitive decline advances [50]. When analyzing the longitudinal studies, we did not find an association of napping with global cognition or memory in the general population. These results suggest that napping does not seem to modify cognitive functions in longitudinal studies regardless of whether the trajectory of cognition is not the same for all adults, which has been previously described to have a negative influence on the cognitive function and sleep disturbances associated with aging. [55, 56]

Although our data indicate no association of napping with cognitive functions, several aspects might be considered when analyzing this relationship. The high prevalence of chronic diseases among the elderly population may be related to daily sleepiness induced by medication or fatigue [57, 58]. Additionally, the frequency of napping might be considered since some differences might be found when napping several short times during the day instead of taking an isolated longer nap. In this sense, naps may have a compensatory function when night sleep is fragmented or not completely restorative [59]. Finally, the relationship between napping as a sedentary behavior and isolation might be considered since aging is associated with an increase in the depression incidence, which may be fostered by inactivity and isolation [60, 61]. Unexpectedly, this systematic review and meta-analysis could not address these issues. Further studies addressing these issues are needed to better understand the relationship between napping and cognitive functions.

Some neurobiological mechanisms have been proposed to explain the positive effects of napping on cognition. Awake periods increase amyloid-B accumulation in the brain extracellular space, which is a peptide that interferes with synaptic activity and may be cleared during sleep periods [62]. Additionally, reduced neural activity during napping is proposed to relax oxidative processes and vascular demands, fostering the clearance of waste products [49]. Last, sleep periods have been described as essential to consolidate memory. Conversely, negative effects of napping have also been reported. Napping has been related to higher levels of inflammatory markers, including IL-6 levels, which may induce cognitive impairment [63]. Furthermore, napping after lunch may disrupt circadian rhythms at the day-point of best coordination and fastest reaction time [50]. This systematic review and meta-analysis could not confirm any of these positive or negative statements.

This systematic review and meta-analysis suffers from specific limitations that may be highlighted. First, the considerable heterogeneity reported in meta-analyses might reduce the stability of our results and conclusions. Second, language restrictions could generate some risk of bias in the results. Third, the studies included in this systematic review and meta-analysis may differ in the methods used to collect data on napping and cognitive function; additionally, substantial inconsistency was found for the analyses. Fourth, cause-effect relationships could not be inferred from the cross-sectional analysis. Fifth, the influence of some important variables and nap characteristics could not be explored, as they were not reported by the original studies. Sixth, whether napping is an underlying sleep disorder or comorbidity or whether the estimated associations differ based on comorbidities could not be determined in this systematic review and meta-analysis. Seventh, nap determination was based mostly on questionnaires; therefore, differences from the actigraphy data could not be determined. Eight, only data for global cognition and memory could be explored, information on other cognitive domains would be of interest. Ninth, publication bias and modification in the pooled ORs were detected after sensitivity analyses, therefore our results might be considered with caution. Finally, only one study reported more than one follow-up measurement; thus, we could not examine the trajectory of the relationship between napping and cognitive decline over time.

This systematic review and meta-analysis following statistical procedures, was proposed to cover part of the need to clarify the relationships between napping and health outcomes. [18] Our data confirm the lack of an association between napping and global cognition and memory. Additionally, such associations are not modified by individual characteristics, including age and the percentage of women in the sample, or sleep characteristics. This information might be considered when providing lifestyle recommendations to adults with and without cognitive complaints. Further studies considering key variables that may influence such associations, including depression, chronic diseases, and the number of breaks for napping, are needed.

Availability of data and materials

Data from this paper are available from other researches upon request. Alvarez-Bueno C should be contacted if someone wants to request the data from this study.

References

  1. Jaul E, Barron J. Age-related diseases and clinical and public health implications for the 85 years old and over population. Front Public Health. 2017;5:335.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Ahmad FB, Anderson RN. The leading causes of death in the US for 2020. JAMA. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2021.5469.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Zucchella C, Sinforiani E, Tamburin S, et al. The multidisciplinary approach to Alzheimer’s disease and dementia. A narrative review of non-pharmacological treatment. Front Neurol. 2018;9:1058.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Douglas S, James I, Ballard C. Non-pharmacological interventions in dementia. Adv Psychiatr Treat. 2004;10:171–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Lee S-H, Han K, Cho H, et al. Variability in metabolic parameters and risk of dementia: a nationwide population-based study. Alzheimer’s Res Ther. 2018;10:1–13.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Scullin MK, Bliwise DL. Sleep, cognition, and normal aging: integrating a half century of multidisciplinary research. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015;10:97–137.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Stanley N. The physiology of sleep and the impact of ageing. Eur Urol Suppl. 2005;3:17–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Dhand R, Sohal H. Good sleep, bad sleep! The role of daytime naps in healthy adults. Curr Opin Pulm Med. 2006;12:379–82.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Hays JC, Blazer DG, Foley DJ. Risk of napping: excessive daytime sleepiness and mortality in an older community population. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1996;44:693–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Yamada T, Shojima N, Yamauchi T, Kadowaki T. J-curve relation between daytime nap duration and type 2 diabetes or metabolic syndrome: a dose-response meta-analysis. Sci Rep. 2016;6(1):1–10.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Owusu JT, Wennberg AM, Holingue CB, et al. Napping characteristics and cognitive performance in older adults. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2019;34(1):87–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Milner CE, Cote KA. Benefits of napping in healthy adults: impact of nap length, time of day, age, and experience with napping. J Sleep Res. 2009;18(2):272–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Shi L, Chen S-J, Ma M-Y, et al. Sleep disturbances increase the risk of dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med Rev. 2018;40:4–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Milner CE, Cote KA. Benefits of napping in healthy adults: impact of nap length, time of day, age, and experience with napping. J Sleep Res. 2009;18:272–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Kåreholt I, Lennartsson C, Gatz M, Parker MG. Baseline leisure time activity and cognition more than two decades later. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2011;26:65–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Jung K-I, Song C-H, Ancoli-Israel S, Barrett-Connor E. Gender differences in nighttime sleep and daytime napping as predictors of mortality in older adults: the Rancho Bernardo study. Sleep Med. 2013;14:12–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Yoon I-Y, Kripke DF, Elliott JA, Youngstedt SD, Rex KM, Hauger RL. Age-related changes of circadian rhythms and sleep-wake cycles. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2003;51:1085–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Zhang Z, Xiao X, Ma W, Li J. Napping in older adults: a review of current literature. Curr Sleep Med Rep. 2020;6(3):1–7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, et al. Updating guidance for reporting systematic reviews: development of the PRISMA 2020 statement. J Clin Epidemiol. 2021;134:103–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Chandler J, Cumpston M, Thomas J, Higgins JPT, Deeks JJ, Clarke MJ. Chapter I: Introduction. In: Higgins JPT, Thomas J, Chandler J, Cumpston M, Li T, Page MJ, Welch VA (editors). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions version 6.2 (updated February 2021). Cochrane. 2021.

  21. National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. Quality assessment tool for observational cohort and cross-sectional studies. https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-topics/study-quality-assessment-tools (31 Jan 2018, date last accessed).

  22. DerSimonian R, Kacker R. Random-effects model for meta-analysis of clinical trials: an update. Contemp Clin Trials. 2007;28:105–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. IntHout J, Ioannidis JPA, Borm GF. The Hartung-Knapp-Sidik-Jonkman method for random effects meta-analysis is straightforward and considerably outperforms the standard DerSimonian-Laird method. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2014;14:1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Higgins JPT, Thompson SG. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat Med. 2002;21:1539–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Higgins JPT, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ. 2003;327:557–60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Stettler C, Allemann S, Wandel S, et al. Drug eluting and bare metal stents in people with and without diabetes: collaborative network meta-analysis. BMJ. 2008;337: a1331.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Lipsey MW, Wilson DB. Practical Meta-Analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Peterson RA, Brown SP. On the use of beta coefficients in meta-analysis. J Appl Psychol. 2005;90:175–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Sterne JA, Egger M, Smith GD. Systematic reviews in health care: investigating and dealing with publication and other biases in meta-analysis. BMJ. 2001;323:101–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Auyeung TW, Lee JSW, Leung J, et al. Cognitive deficit is associated with phase advance of sleep-wake rhythm, daily napping, and prolonged sleep duration–a cross-sectional study in 2,947 community-dwelling older adults. Age. 2013;35:479–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Blackwell T, Yaffe K, Ancoli-Israel S, et al. Poor sleep is associated with impaired cognitive function in older women: the study of osteoporotic fractures. J Gerontol Biol Sci Med Sci. 2006;61:405–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Cai H, Su N, Li W, Li X, Xiao S, Sun L. Relationship between afternoon napping and cognitive function in the ageing Chinese population. Gen Psychiatry. 2021;34: e100361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Chiu HY, Lai FC, Chen PY, Tsai PS. Differences between men and women aged 65 and older in the relationship between self-reported sleep and cognitive impairment: a nationwide survey in Taiwan. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2016;64:2051–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Coelho L, Hauck K, McKenzie K, et al. The association between sedentary behavior and cognitive ability in older adults. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2020;32:2339–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Cross N, Terpening Z, Rogers NL, et al. Napping in older people ‘at risk’ of dementia: relationships with depression, cognition, medical burden and sleep quality. J Sleep Res. 2015;24:494–502.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Fang SC, Huang CJ, Wu YL, Wu PY, Tsai PS. Effects of napping on cognitive function modulation in elderly adults with a morning chronotype: a nationwide survey. J Sleep Res. 2018;28: e12724.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Frisoni GB, Leo DD, Rozzini R, Trabucchi M. Napping in the elderly and its association with night sleep and psychological status. Int Psychogeriatr. 1996;8:477–87.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Li J, Cacchione PZ, Hodgson N, et al. Afternoon napping and cognition in chinese older adults: findings from the china health and retirement longitudinal study baseline assessment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2017;65:373–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Lin J-F, Li F-D, Chen X-G, et al. Association of postlunch napping duration and night-time sleep duration with cognitive impairment in Chinese elderly: a cross-sectional study. BMJ Open. 2018;8: e023188.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Ohayon MM, Vecchierini MF. Daytime sleepiness and cognitive impairment in the elderly population. Arch Intern Med. 2002;162:201–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Owusu JT, Wennberg AMV, Holingue CB, Tzuang M, Abeson KD, Spira AP. Napping characteristics and cognitive performance in older adults. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2019;34:87–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Qian YX, Ma QH, Sun HP, Xu Y, Pan CW. Combined effect of three common lifestyle factors on cognitive impairment among older Chinese adults: a community-based, cross-sectional survey. Psychogeriatrics. 2020;20:844–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Sagherian K, Rose K. Long work hours, prolonged daytime naps, and decreased cognitive performance in older adults. Chronobiol Int. 2020;37:1304–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Wang Q, Zhu H, Dai R, et al. Associations between total sleep duration and cognitive function among middle-aged and older chinese adults: does midday napping have an effect on it? International Journal of General Medicine. 2022;15:1381.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Xie X, He J, Du J, Chen Y, et al. Cognitive function and associated factors in older adults: evidence from the China health and retirement longitudinal study. Psychogeriatrics. 2022;22(3):308–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Xin C, Zhang B, Fang S, Zhou J. Daytime napping and successful aging among older adults in China: a cross-sectional study. BMC Geriatr. 2020;20:1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Xu L, Jiang CQ, Lam TH, et al. Short or long sleep duration is associated with memory impairment in older Chinese: the Guangzhou biobank cohort study. Sleep. 2011;34:575–80.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Cox SR, Ritchie SJ, Allerhand M, et al. Sleep and cognitive aging in the eighth decade of life. Sleep. 2019;42:zsz019.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Dzierzewski JM, Fung CH, Jouldjian S, Alessi CA, Irwin MR, Martin JL. Decrease in daytime sleeping is associated with improvement in cognition after hospital discharge in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2014;62:47–53.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Keage HAD, Banks S, Yang KL, Morgan K, Brayne C, Matthews FE. What sleep characteristics predict cognitive decline in the elderly? Sleep Med. 2012;13:886–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Kitamura K, Watanabe Y, Nakamura K, et al. Short daytime napping reduces the risk of cognitive decline in community-dwelling older adults: a 5-year longitudinal study. BMC Geriatr. 2021;21(1):1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Leng Y, Redline S, Stone KL, Ancoli-Israel S, Yaffe K. Objective napping, cognitive decline, and risk of cognitive impairment in older men. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2019;15:1039–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Li J, Chang YP, Riegel B, et al. Intermediate, but not extended, afternoon naps may preserve cognition in chinese older adults. J Gerontol Biol Sci Med Sci. 2018;73:360–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Sha T, Cheng W, Yan Y. Prospective association between sleep-related factors and the trajectories of cognitive performance in the elderly Chinese population across a 5-year period cohort study. PLoS ONE. 2019;14: e0222192.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Ohayon MM, Vecchierini MF. Normative sleep data, cognitive function and daily living activities in older adults in the community. Sleep. 2005;28:981–9.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Mander BA, Winer JR, Walker MP. Sleep and human aging. Neuron. 2017;94:19–36.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Zhou J, Kessler AS, Su D. Association between daytime napping and chronic diseases in China. Am J Health Behav. 2016;40:182–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Léger D, Torres MJ, Bayon V, et al. The association between physical and mental chronic conditions and napping. Sci Rep. 2019;9:1–7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  59. Xu W, Tan CC, Zou JJ, Cao XP, Tan L. Sleep problems and risk of all-cause cognitive decline or dementia: an updated systematic review and meta-analysis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2020;91:236–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Liu Y, Peng T, Zhang S, Tang K. The relationship between depression, daytime napping, daytime dysfunction, and snoring in 0.5 million Chinese populations: exploring the effects of socio-economic status and age. BMC Public Health. 2018;18:1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Mammen G, Faulkner G. Physical activity and the prevention of depression. Am J Prev Med. 2013;45:649–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Kang JE, Lim MM, Bateman RJ, et al. Amyloid-beta dynamics are regulated by orexin and the sleep-wake cycle. Science. 2009;326:1005–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Basta M, Koutentaki E, Vgontzas A, et al. Objective daytime napping is associated with disease severity and inflammation in patients with mild to moderate dementia. J Alzheimers Dis. 2020;74:803–15.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This study was funded by European Regional Development Fund.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

CAB conceptualized and designed the study with the support of AEM and VMV. CAB drafted the initial manuscript and, along with VMV and AEM, approved the final manuscript as submitted. CAB, AEM, VMV, ASL and EJL designed the data collection protocol and instruments and coordinated and supervised data collection. CAB, AEM, SRG, ASL, EJL, and VMV were involved in the analysis and interpretation of the data, reviewed and revised the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Arthur Eumann Mesas.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

None declared.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

We certify that this work is novel and provides a consistent summary of the evidence for the effects of napping on cognitive decline.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1:

Supplementary Table 1. PRISMA checklist. Supplementary Table 2. Search strategy for Medline. Supplementary Figure 1. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews flowchart. Supplementary Table 3. Characteristics of the Cross-Sectional Studies Included in the Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis on the Association Between Napping and Cognition Parameters. Supplementary Table 4. Characteristics of the Longitudinal Studies Included in the Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis on the Association Between Napping and Cognition Parameters. Supplementary Table 5. Covariates used to adjust the analyses reported by the included studies. Supplementary Table 6. Risk of bias of cross-sectional and longitudinal included studies. Supplementary Table 7. Inconsistence and heterogeneity estimations for DerSimoniand and Lair and Hartung-Knapp-Sidik-Jonkman random effects methods. Supplementary Table 8. Meta-regression of napping and cognition domains by percentage of females and mean age of included studies. Supplementary Table 9. Meta-regression of napping and cognition domains by percentage of no nappers, percentage of people included in the less nighttime sleep duration category and mean night sleep time (hours) included studies. Supplementary Table 10. Sensitivity analyses by removing studies one by one for cross-sectional analysis. Supplementary Table 11. Sensitivity analyses by removing studies one by one for longitudinal analysis. Supplementary Table 12. Meta-bias for the association between IMT and cognitive function domains.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Álvarez-Bueno, C., Mesas, A.E., Reina-Gutierrez, S. et al. Napping and cognitive decline: a systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. BMC Geriatr 22, 756 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-022-03436-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-022-03436-2

Keywords