Skip to main content

The effect of co-dependent (thinking in motion [TIM]) versus single-modality (CogniFit) interventions on cognition and gait among community-dwelling older adults with cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled study

Abstract

Background

Cognition and motor skills are interrelated throughout the aging process and often show simultaneous deterioration among older adults with cognitive impairment. Co-dependent training has the potential to ameliorate both domains; however, its effect on the gait and cognition of older adults with cognitive impairment has yet to be explored. The aim of this study is to compare the effects of the well-established single-modality cognitive computerized training program, CogniFit, with “Thinking in Motion (TIM),” a co-dependent group intervention, among community-dwelling older adults with cognitive impairment.

Methods

Employing a single-blind randomized control trial design, 47 community-dwelling older adults with cognitive impairment were randomly assigned to 8 weeks of thrice-weekly trainings of TIM or CogniFit. Pre- and post-intervention assessments included cognitive performance, evaluated by a CogniFit battery, as a primary outcome; and gait, under single- and dual-task conditions, as a secondary outcome.

Results

CogniFit total Z scores significantly improved from baseline to post-intervention for both groups. There was a significant main effect for time [F (1, 44) = 17.43, p < .001, ηp2 = .283] but not for group [F (1, 44) = 0.001, p = .970]. No time X group interaction [F (1, 44) = 1.29, p = .261] was found. No changes in gait performance under single and dual-task performance were observed in both groups.

Conclusions

The findings show that single-modality (CogniFit) and co-dependent (TIM) trainings improve cognition but not gait in older adults with cognitive impairment. Such investigations should be extended to include various populations and a broader set of outcome measurements.

Trial registration

ACTRN12616001543471. Date: 08/11/2016.

Peer Review reports

Literature review

The older adult population is expected to grow 6–7% per year between 2025 and 2029, resulting in a quadrupling in this century [1]. The presence of dementia in people aged 45 and over in Israel in 2016 was 2.5% of the total population. For those aged 65 and over, the presence was 6.4% and for those aged 85 and over – 22% [2]. Cognitive impairment is associated with an increased risk of disability, higher risk of dementia [3], reduced gait quality and increased risk of falls [3,4,5,6,7,8]. The relationship between cognition and gait quality is so strong that baseline gait speed is used to predict changes in cognition such as processing speed, memory and executive function [9]. Owing to the interplay between motor and cognitive aging, both cognitive and motor interventions have reciprocal positive effects among older adults. Such interventions can be classified as single-modality (motor or cognitive) or combined training (motor and cognitive) [6, 8]. Indeed, single-modality cognitive intervention shows transfer effects to motor abilities [5] and vice versa [10]. However, the multi-sensory simultaneous reinforcement of the combined multi-modality training may have a greater effect, especially since cognition, motor skills, sensory and temporal processing share neural substrates and functional processes [11]. In addition, older adults appear to use their brains more integrally than young people, as suggested by Harold’s theory [12], and it is possible that this brain behavior present subsequent compensation mechanisms to aid a poor one-modal processing. Multisensory stimulation could, therefore, enhance multi-modal cognitive processing, and especially contribute to low-functioning older adults who suffer from multi-modal impairment, enabling them to rely on more than one domain [9]. The combined multi-modality motor-cognitive interventions can be divided into three types based on the relationships between tasks: (1) independent performance of serial motor and cognitive tasks (either in the same session or in separate sessions), a sequential training shown to be effective mainly when an aerobic motor part precedes the cognitive part of the training [13]; (2) simultaneous performance of tasks i.e., Dual-Task (DT), aiming to improve mainly divided attention [4], found to be effective in improving various motor (e.g., balance and postural control) and cognitive functions (e.g., executive function, memory, and attention) during single and DT conditions [14] (however, a systematic review argues that this training has limited transferability [13]); and (3) simultaneous performance of co-dependent tasks, i.e., an integration of cognitive and motor demands embedded within one task (which cannot be executed separately as in DT), like walking while navigating [15].

Co-dependent tasks represent the requirements of many daily life tasks and are more ecological than other combined trainings such as DT [15]. Although understudied compared to the other two types, co-dependent interventions showed improvement in global cognition [16], executive control, processing speed [17], and balance and motor performance [18,19,20,21] among community-dwelling older adults. Evidence also suggests that there is a reduction of the risk of falls following co-dependent training. It seems that the approach of motor and cognitive stimulation allows these two domains to be strengthened not only separately but also as a uniform execution unit [21]. Nevertheless, the potential contribution of co-dependent training to older adults with cognitive impairment is yet to be determined [22].

According to the patient-centered care approach, matching between interventions and specific patients’ needs can optimize the interventions’ effects [23]. Moreover, a simultaneous reinforcement of multisensory systems, as expressed in the co-dependent training, can lead to a greater effect on various systems compared to a single modality training, especially among older adults [9]. To address this question among community-dwelling older adults with cognitive impairment, we developed a co-dependent group training with high adaptability to a variety of cognitive levels called “Thinking in Motion – TIM” [24], and compared its effect on cognition and gait performance to the well-established cognitive computerized training, i.e., CogniFit, among community-dwelling older adults aged 65 and above.

Our primary hypothesis was that the TIM intervention will be as effective as CogniFit in improving global cognition, and more specifically in domains such as working memory, divided attention, processing speed, and visual scanning. These domains were intensively trained in TIM intervention and were shown to be improved after CogniFit training [25]. In addition, considering the specificity of the training of gross motor skills in the TIM intervention, our secondary hypothesis was that TIM will have a higher effect on gait speed and variability during single task (ST) and DT compared to CogniFit.

Methods

Eligibility and study design

A single-blind, randomized, and controlled clinical trial compared the effects of TIM, a co-dependent motor-cognitive intervention, and CogniFit, a single modality computerized cognitive training (control), on cognition and gait quality in 47 community-dwelling older adults who participate on a daily basis in leisure activities at the adult day center. Sample size was based on G power calculation with the following parameters: Two groups, measured at two time points; effect size used was the mean Cohen’s d of the group differences post intervention of the 16 CogniFit measures in Haimov & Shatil (2013), which was 0.635 (Cohen’s ƒ = 0.318); α was 0.05; power to detect an effect was 95%; correlations between time points were assumed to be 0.2. Potential participants were recruited through referrals from day center staff members. Inclusion criteria were: (1) age over 65; (2) ability to walk independently with or without an assistive device; (3) ability to understand simple instructions and sign an informed consent form; (4) ability to commit to the program based on a short interview. This set of minimum criteria seeks to represent as reliably as possible the population of the day centers. Subjects with a MoCA [26] score lower than 18 underwent a follow-up interview to examine their eligibility to participate in the study. Participants who met the inclusion criteria were randomly assigned to an eight-week, thrice per week, 40-minute intervention in one of two groups: a TIM group intervention (n = 28), which was split into smaller training groups that contained 14 practitioners in each group; or an individual computerized cognitive training group using CogniFit training (n = 19) (i.e., control group). Randomization was conducted with a concealed allocation using a table with a random number that was generated by a statistician. Levene’s test of homogeneity of variance was not significant for any of the measures, and Box’s M test of equality of covariance matrices was not significant for any of the repeated measures ANOVA tests. Baseline assessment was conducted by a research assistant, naive to participant assignment, and included: (1) demographic and psychological measures: The Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) [27], Short Anxiety Screening Test (SAST) [28] and the Activities-specific Balance Confidence (ABC) Scale [29]; (2) cognitive performance measures: Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA [26] and a computerized neuro-cognitive assessment using CogniFit battery [30]; and (3) gait assessment in single and dual-task conditions using an accelerometer and gyroscope mounted on the waist from McRoberts Mobility Lab [31]. Cognitive and gait evaluations were conducted again after 8 weeks of intervention. The study protocol was approved by an institutional review board. All participants signed a consent form and did not receive any monetary compensation for participation.

Thinking in motion (TIM) intervention

TIM is a combined motor-cognitive co-dependent intervention inspired by the Eshkol-Wachman Movement Notation (EWMN), which uses graphic symbols to describe motion [32]. Unlike the fixed use of graphic symbols in EWMN, however, TIM departs from the permanent system of symbols and uses the ephemerality of symbols and frequent changes in them as a central tool for cognitive stimulation. The ephemerality and frequent changes require a renewed learning process, coping with coding permanence and mental flexibility. In addition, the use of symbols creates the cognitive challenge of producing movement from interpreting a graphic sign, i.e., visual scanning, information processing, and spatial perception. The symbols represent various components in movement such as organ movement, landmarks in space, time, and rhythm elements. After establishing the given symbols for the exercise, a cognitive challenge is graded by a constant change of the symbols’ order. The TIM trainer uses a “manipulation bank” that is applied to various components of the movement or how they are presented graphically. The chosen manipulations enable the TIM trainer to manage the level of difficulty and the variety of cognitive skills required to perform them. An example of TIM training and manipulation options is shown in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Example of manipulation option in TIM

Control group- CogniFit intervention

CogniFit is a computerized cognitive training program which was found to be effective in improving cognition and gait [7, 25]. The program was personally tailored based on a 45-minute baseline assessment [30]. Each training session included a mixture of auditory, visual, and cross-modality tasks aimed at training executive functions, attention, and other cognitive processes. Verbal instructions were written on the screen before each task and then demonstrated by the program [33]. Technical support in operating the software and assistance in understanding the tasks were available.

Outcome measures assessments

Measures were undertaken at baseline and after the eight-week intervention. All assessments were conducted by a blinded research assistant.

Primary outcome: cognitive functions

The 40-minute CogniFit computerized neurocognitive assessment battery [30] was used to evaluate changes in cognitive function following both interventions. Apart from the global score, the cognitive indicators examined in this study were working memory, divided attention, processing speed, and visual scanning. These tests were found to have good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.85–0.88), and test-retest reliability (r = 0.69–0.92). The CogniFit assessment battery also has validity, as it is based on well validated cognitive tests [34].

Secondary outcome: gait performance

Gait measures included gait speed and stride time variability collected under ST and DT conditions with the McRoberts Mobility Lab, an accelerometer and gyroscope attached to the participants’ waists [35] which measured temporal variables. Gait with DT is a well-established marker to describe progression of dementia and cognitive impairment. The link between cognition and gait control has been sufficiently demonstrated [4]. Gait tasks took place for periods of 1 min along a six-meter route in a quiet room. Assessment started in a static position, and the participants were instructed to walk at their usual pace. One trial for each conditions (single and dual task) was performed. Turn periods were discarded, and gait variability was calculated using the total number of strides by dividing standard deviation for the gait variable by its mean. With according to previous studies [36, 37], the mean and the standard deviation of gait cycles’ number used to measure gait variability was 29.72 ± 9.57 for single task, and 24.89 ± 10.91 for dual task. The cognitive task attached to the walking was subtraction by 3 from a random number between 100 and 250 [38]. The order of the tasks was randomized.

Analysis

All analyses were performed using SPSS 26 (IBM SPSS Statistics, New York, US). Continuous data are presented as mean and standard deviation or median and interquartile range, while categorical data are presented as frequencies (percentage and number of participants). Between-group differences in demographic data were analyzed via Mann-Whitney U tests, Pearsons’ chi-square test, or Fisher’s Exact tests. Global and domain-specific CogniFit measures, presented as mean ± standard deviation at baseline and post-intervention were normalized (z-scored) to normative data of the population obtained from the CogniFit database, and were examined using between-within repeated measures Analysis of variance (ANOVA). When an interaction was significant, it was followed by t-test post-hoc analyses. Gait outcomes were speed and stride time variability. Effect size estimator was partial eta-squared (ηp2) for the ANOVA test. Effect sizes are reported for significant comparisons only. Data deviating more than |2.5| standard deviation from group mean were considered outliers and were excluded from statistical analysis. Data were analyzed with an intention-to-treat approach.

Results

Group characteristics

A total of 54 participants were referred to the research team by the center staff. Seven participants withdrew from the study before group assignment: five did not meet the inclusion criteria and two declined to participate (Fig. 2). Thus, the sample was comprised of 30 women and 17 men, with a mean age of 81.16 years (SD = 8.23). No differences between groups in age, years of education, gender, smoking, cardio-metabolic diseases, stroke, balance efficacy, anxiety, depression, and cognitive abilities at baseline were found (Table 1). It is notable that the participants in this sample were frail older adults with low gait speed and MoCA scores that are associated with moderate to severe cognitive impairment. No subjects were diagnosed with dementia; however, as noted above, subjects with a MoCA [26] score lower than 18 underwent a follow-up interview to examine their eligibility to participate in the study.

Fig. 2
figure 2

CONSORT Flow of participants

Table 1 Comparisons of demographic measures between the groups at baseline

The means and standard deviations of the groups in the study’s outcome measures are presented in Table 2.

Table 2 Means and standard deviations of the study’s outcome measures

Primary outcomes

Results are presented in Fig. 3. Compared with baseline, both groups improved in cognition as measured by the CogniFit test. Analysis of CogniFit total score (Z-scored) revealed a significant main effect for time [F (1, 44) = 17.43, p < .001, ηp2 = .283] such that the cognitive performance at post-intervention (M = -3.31, SD = 1.40) improved significantly in both groups, compared to the cognitive performance at baseline (M = -3.89, SD = 1.49), with a large effect size. No main effect for group [F (1, 44) = 0.001, p = .970], or a time X group interaction [F (1, 44) = 1.29, p = .261] were found. Similar results were demonstrated in specific cognitive domains: working memory following intervention (M = -3.4, SD = 1.36) improved compared to baseline (M = -4.00, SD = 1.63), with a large effect size [F (1, 44) = 10.97, p = .001, ηp2 = .199], with no main effect for group or a time X group interaction. Divided attention improved following intervention with a medium effect size [F (1, 44) = 5.54, p = .023, ηp2 = .111] from (M = -2.28, SD = 0.50) to (M = -2.05, SD = 0.61). No main effect for group was found, but the time X group interaction [F (1, 44) = 3.90, p = .054] was closer to being statistically significant. Processing speed post-intervention (M = -3.87, SD = 2.24) improved compared with baseline (M = -4.50, SD = 2.38) with a medium effect size [F (1, 41) = 5.73, p = .021, ηp2 = .122], and no effect for group or time X group interaction were found. In contrast, in the visual scanning domain, a significant time X group interaction emerged [F (1, 44) = 4.63, p = .036, ηp2 = .095] with a medium effect size. Post-hoc analysis revealed that while there was no difference between the groups at baseline [t (44)= − 0.13, p = .893], there was a significant difference at post-intervention [t (44)= − 2.11, p = .039], so the TIM group (M = -1.63, SD = 2.03) improved more than CogniFit (M = -2.96, SD = 2.17). Paired-samples t-tests showed no difference within the groups (not shown).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Effect of interventions on global cognition and on specific domains. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Secondary outcomes

Analyses of gait speed under ST condition revealed no main effect for time [F (1, 42) = 0.02, p = .876] or group [F (1, 42) = 1.60, p = .212], and no significant time X group interaction [F (1, 42) = 2.23, p = .142]. On the other hand, under DT condition, a main effect for group was found [F (1, 43) = 5.36, p = .025, ηp2 = 0.11], so the control group had a higher mean gait speed (M = 0.41, SD = 0.15) compared to the intervention group (M = 0.31 m / seconds, SD = 0.15) at both time points. No main effect for time [F (1, 43) = 0.05, p = .816] or time X group interaction [F (1, 43) = 0.02, p = .869] were found. Variability of stride time under ST did not differ between or within the groups, so no effects for time [F (1, 38) = 0.22, p = .635], group [F (1, 38) = 1.34, p = .253], or interaction between them [F (1, 38) = 0.005, p = .942] were found. Variability of stride time under DT also did not differ between or within the groups, so no effects for time [F (1, 37) = 0.35, p = .555], group [F (1, 37) = 1.06, p = .307], or interaction between them [F (1, 37) = 0.63, p = .429] were found.

Discussion

The findings of this single-blind randomized clinical trial demonstrate cognitive improvement in both interventions, i.e., TIM and CogniFit, among community-dwelling older adults with cognitive impairment. The intervention group average performance was 82.5% of all the sessions that took place. In the control group, 90% of the participants received assistance in connection with the training software program. However, their adherence was low, with completion rate at around 30%. Most of our findings support the first hypothesis, suggesting similar global cognitive improvement, as well as similar improvement in specific domains, i.e., working memory, divided attention, and processing speed among both groups. These findings are in line with previous findings [16, 17, 20, 25, 39]. One exception is the visual scanning domain, which, in contrast to our hypothesis, was improved only among TIM participants. Indeed, visual scanning is a central requirement within TIM training due to its extensive use of graphical tools. Unlike our findings, a previous study by Shatil showed improvement in visual scanning following 8 weeks of CogniFit training among community-dwelling older adults [25]. This discrepancy can be explained by the different cognitive abilities of the participants in the current study and Shatil’s research [25], probably thus limiting the participants in the control group (CogniFit) from benefiting from its advantages. Notwithstanding, such cognitive results are encouraging, especially given the participants’ relatively low baseline cognitive scores [40], and further support the potential for cognitive improvement even among older adults with cognitive impairments [41]. Such potential is supported by the remedial model that stresses the ability of training to reinforce brain plasticity [42, 43].

Our second hypothesis, that both groups would show motor improvement with an advantage for the TIM group, was not supported and no significant improvement in gait performance was observed among participants in either group. These findings are inconsistent with a previous study that demonstrated gait improvement after thrice-weekly, eight-week CogniFit training among high-functioning older adults [7] and with studies that evaluated the effect of various co-dependent trainings on various older adult populations [18, 20, 39, 44, 45]. The difference between findings may stem from the differences in baseline cognitive statuses between the participants in the above-mentioned studies. The learning abilities of those with cognitive impairment are limited [41]. In the current study, due to safety reasons the intervention was conducted while sitting, similar to the control group, yet both groups maintained their gait abilities during the intervention period. Indeed, maintenance of abilities is often the main goal with older adults with cognitive impairment [46].

This study contained several limitations. The first is that although the composition of the population in the study faithfully represents the composition of the population that reaches the day centers, this also has research limitations. The sample is homogenous in several aspects such as socio-economic status and general functioning, but the cognitive score is widely varied. Indeed, the objective cognitive score is associated with various factors. While this wide range of cognitive function may represent the general population, it can confound the findings and limit their generalizability to different populations such as higher functioning older adults or patients with neurological diseases. Second, due to the small sample, we did not control for gender in the analysis. Third, the cognitive assessment was conducted using the CogniFit assessment tool which was similar to the training of CogniFit. Yet, the fact that both groups similarly improved in this test emphasizes the transferability of TIM training. Fourth, the motor effects of the interventions were examined only on gait in a laboratory setting, limiting their transferability to other daily functions. Fifth, it is hard to infer a causal pathway between the interventions and the preserved gait performance, due to the lack of a waiting list control group. Sixth, due to the limited number of participants and limited resources, unbalanced randomization was conducted and the intervention group was larger than the control group; however, given that intervention effectiveness for this group has been examined in previous studies, correction was not made. Seventh, the effect of the difference between settings (individual vs. group) could not be controlled and the adherence rate was higher in the intervention group. Future studies should be conducted among various populations that explore broader motor and social abilities (e.g., participation) in ecological settings and incorporate a waiting list control arm.

Conclusions

Our findings demonstrate that among community-dwelling older adults with cognitive impairment, 8 weeks of thrice-weekly interventions (TIM and CogniFit) may contribute to global cognition, working memory, divided attention, and processing speed. TIM also demonstrated an improvement in visual scanning. No change in gait performance was observed among participants in both groups. Older adults can benefit from the advantages of both interventions, supporting personalization of treatment plans. Future studies should evaluate TIM among a broader population of older adults in different settings to further address the effect of co-dependent combined training compared to a single modality intervention on motor abilities and cognition throughout the aging process.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request. The study is registered in ANZCTR, under registration number ACTRN12616001543471, where the full protocol is available.

Abbreviations

TIM:

Thinking In Motion

DT:

Dual Task

ST:

Single Task

References

  1. Brunborg H. Increasing life expectancy and the growing elderly population. Nor Epidemiol. 2012;22:75–83.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Dementia Survey (Hebrew). https://www.health.gov.il/PublicationsFiles/dementia-report_2008-2019.pdf.

  3. Plassman BL, et al. Prevalence of cognitive impairment without dementia in the United States. Ann Intern Med. 2008;148:427–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Clark DJ. Automaticity of walking: functional significance, mechanisms, measurement and rehabilitation strategies. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2015;9(246):1-13.

  5. Kennedy KM, Raz N. Age, sex and regional brain volumes predict perceptual-motor skill acquisition. Cortex. 2005;41:560–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Zhu X, Yin S, Lang M, He R. The more the better? A meta-analysis on effects of combined cognitive and physical intervention on cognition in healthy older adults. Ageing Res Rev. 2016;31:67–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Verghese J, Mahoney J, Ambrose AF, Wang C, Holtzer R. Effect of cognitive remediation on gait in sedentary seniors. J Gerontol Ser A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2010;65A:1338–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Bamidis PD, et al. A review of physical and cognitive interventions in aging. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;44:206–20.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Paraskevoudi N, Balcı F, Vatakis A. “Walking” through the sensory, cognitive, and temporal degradations of healthy aging. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2018;1426:72–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Etgen T, et al. Physical activity and incident cognitive impairment in elderly persons. Arch Intern Med. 2010;170:186.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Parihar R, Mahoney JR, Verghese J. Relationship of gait and cognition in the elderly. Curr Transl Geriatr Exp Gerontol Rep. 2013;2:167–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Cabeza R. Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model. Psychol Aging. 2002;17:85–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Law LLF, Barnett F, Yau MK, Gray MA. Effects of combined cognitive and exercise interventions on cognition in older adults with and without cognitive impairment: a systematic review. Ageing Res Rev. 2014;15:61–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Agmon M, Belza B, Nguyen HQ, Logsdon RG, Kelly VE. A systematic review of interventions conducted in clinical or community settings to improve dual-task postural control in older adults. Clin Interv Aging. 2014;9:477–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Herold F, Hamacher D, Schega L, Müller NG. Thinking while moving or moving while thinking - concepts of motor-cognitive training for cognitive performance enhancement. Front Aging Neurosci. 2018;10:228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Rosenberg D, Depp CA, Vahia IV, Reichstadt J, Palmer BW, Kerr J, et al. Exergames for subsyndromal depression in older adults: a pilot study of a novel intervention. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;18:221–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Maillot P, Perrot A, Aging AH-P. Effects of interactive physical-activity video-game training on physical and cognitive function in older adults. Psychol Aging. 2012;27:589–600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Wong AM, Lin Y-C, Chou S-W, Tang F-T, Wong P-Y. Coordination exercise and postural stability in elderly people: effect of tai chi Chuan. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2001;82:608–12.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Kimura K, Exercise, N. H.-P. of S. and & 2012, U. Investigating the acute effect of an aerobic dance exercise program on neuro-cognitive function in the elderly. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2012;13:623–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Pohl P, Dizdar N, Hallert E. The Ronnie Gardiner Rhythm and Music Method–a feasibility study in Parkinson’s disease. Disability and rehabilitation. 2013;35(26):2197–204.

  21. Mirelman A, Rochester L, Maidan I, Del Din S, Alcock L, Nieuwhof F, et al. Addition of a non-immersive virtual reality component to treadmill training to reduce fall risk in older adults (V-TIME): a randomised controlled trial. Lancet. 2016;388:1170–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Pichierri G, Wolf P, Murer K, De Bruin ED. Cognitive and cognitive-motor interventions affecting physical functioning: a systematic review. BMC Geriatr. 2011;11:1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Ferrari H. Psychodynamic Diagnostic Manual, PDM. Vertex (Buenos Aires, Argentina). 2006;17(69):356–61.

  24. Embon- Magal, S. TIM. Available at: tim-method.com.

  25. Shatil E. Does combined cognitive training and physical activity training enhance cognitive abilities more than either alone? A four-condition randomized controlled trial among healthy older adults. Front Aging Neurosci. 2013;5:1–12.

  26. Nasreddine ZS, et al. The montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53:695–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Nyunt MSZ, Fones C, Niti M, Ng TP. Criterion-based validity and reliability of the geriatric depression screening scale (GDS-15) in a large validation sample of community-living Asian older adults. Aging Ment Heal. 2009;13:376–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Sinoff G, Liora O, Zlotogorsky D, Tamir A. Short anxiety screening test - A brief instrument for detecting anxiety in the elderly. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 1999;14:1062–71.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Powell LE, Myers AM. The activities-specific balance confidence (ABC) scale. Gerontol Ser A Biol Sci Med Sci. 1995;50A:M28–34.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Haimov I, Hanuka E, Horowitz Y. Chronic insomnia and cognitive functioning among older adults. Behav Sleep Med. 2008;6:32–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Lab MM. McRoberts-Moving Technology. Available at: https://www.mcroberts.nl/. (Accessed: 8th July 2021).

  32. Eshkol N, W. A. Movement notation. (Weidenfeld and Fedigan, 1958).

  33. CogniFit. Cognitive Research Platform. Available at: https://www.cognifit.com/cognitive-research-tool.

  34. CogniFit. Validity description of CogniFit assessments. (2016). Available at: https://s3.amazonaws.com/static.cognifit.com/customersupport/Validation+for+Researchers.pdf.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Senden R, Grimm B, Heyligers I, Savelberg H, Meijer K. Acceleration-based gait test for healthy subjects: reliability and reference data. Gait Posture. 2009;30:192–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Brach JS, Perera S, Studenski S, Newman AB. The reliability and validity of measures of gait variability in community-dwelling older adults. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2008;89:2293–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. König N, Singh NB, von Beckerath J, Janke L, Taylor WR. Is gait variability reliable? An assessment of spatio-temporal parameters of gait variability during continuous overground walking. Gait Posture. 2014;39:615–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Agmon M, Kelly VE, Logsdon RG, Nguyen H, Belza B. The effects of Enhancefitness (EF) training on dual-task walking in older adults. J Appl Gerontol. 2015;34:128–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Eggenberger P, Schumacher V, Angst M, Theill N, de Bruin ED. Does multicomponent physical exercise with simultaneous cognitive training boost cognitive performance in older adults? A 6-month randomized controlled trial with a 1-year follow-up. Clin Interv Aging. 2015;10:1335–49.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Narazaki K, et al. Normative data for the Montreal cognitive assessment in a Japanese community-dwelling older population. Neuroepidemiology. 2013;40:23–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Hill NTM, et al. Computerized cognitive training in older adults with mild cognitive impairment or dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2017;174:329–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Ben-Yishay Y, Prigatano G. Cognitive remediation; 1990.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Sharma I, Srivastava J, Kumar A, Mental RS. Cognitive remediation therapy for older adults. J Geriatr Ment Heal. 2016;3:57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Goodway JD, Ozmun JC, Gallahue DL. Understanding motor development: Infants, children, adolescents, adults. Jones & Bartlett Learning; 2019.

  45. Nitz JC, Kuys S, Isles R, Fu S. Is the Wii fit™ a new-generation tool for improving balance, health and well-being? A pilot study. Climacteric. 2010;13:487–91.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Smith-Ray R, Hughes S. Impact of cognitive training on balance and gait in older adults. J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2015;70:357–66.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the subjects for their contribution to our research.

Funding

This work was partially supported by the Israeli Alzheimer’s Association (AMDA) of Israel and Teva Pharmaceuticals. The support was expressed in the funding of the working hours of the research assistant and the intervention trainer. AMDA and Teva did not take any part or role in the study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

SEM- Developed the intervention, designed the study, analyzed the data, and wrote the manuscript. MA- Supervised the study, wrote the manuscript. ID- Supervised the study and reviewed the manuscript. TK- Supported mobility data analysis and reviewed the manuscript. EG- Supported cognitive data analysis and reviewed the manuscript. KA- Supported statistical analysis and reviewed the manuscript. IH- Supported CogniFit intervention and reviewed the manuscript. The author(s) read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Maayan Agmon.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Faculty of Social Welfare and Health Sciences, University of Haifa. All methods were performed in accordance with regulations. All participants signed an informed consent and did not receive any monetary compensation for participation. Study subjects reported general satisfaction with participation in the study, and no injuries or harm were reported.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

TIM is a training method that the author Shiri Embon Magal has developed and practiced. Other authors had no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Embon-Magal, S., Krasovsky, T., Doron, I. et al. The effect of co-dependent (thinking in motion [TIM]) versus single-modality (CogniFit) interventions on cognition and gait among community-dwelling older adults with cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled study. BMC Geriatr 22, 720 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-022-03403-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-022-03403-x

Keywords