Skip to main content

A pilot voxel-based morphometry study of older adults after the PICMOR intervention program

Abstract

Background

Age-related decline in cognitive function, such as executive function, is associated with structural changes in the neural substrates, such as volume reductions in the lateral prefrontal cortex. To prevent or delay age-related changes in cognitive function, cognitive intervention methods that employ social activity, including conversations, have been proposed in some intervention studies. Interestingly, previous studies have consistently reported that verbal fluency ability can be trained by conversation-based interventions in healthy older adults. However, little is known about the neural substrates that underlie the beneficial effect of conversation-based interventions on cognitive function. In this pilot study, we aimed to provide candidate brain regions that are responsible for the enhancement of cognitive function, by analyzing structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data that were additionally obtained from participants in our previous intervention study.

Methods

A voxel-based morphometric analysis was applied to the structural MRI data. In the analysis, the regional brain volume was compared between the intervention group, who participated in a group conversation-based intervention program named Photo-Integrated Conversation Moderated by Robots (PICMOR), and the control group, who joined in a control program based on unstructured free conversations. Furthermore, regions whose volume was positively correlated with an increase in verbal fluency task scores throughout the intervention period were explored.

Results

Results showed that the volume of several regions, including the superior frontal gyrus, parahippocampal gyrus/hippocampus, posterior middle temporal gyrus, and postcentral gyrus, was greater in the intervention group than in the control group. In contrast, no regions showed greater volume in the control group than in the intervention group. The region whose volume showed a positive correlation with the increased task scores was identified in the inferior parietal lobule.

Conclusions

Although definitive conclusions cannot be drawn from this study due to a lack of MRI data from the pre-intervention period, it achieved the exploratory purpose by successfully identifying candidate brain regions that reflect the beneficial effect of conversation-based interventions on cognitive function, including the lateral prefrontal cortex, which plays an important role in executive functions.

Trial registration

The trial was retrospectively registered on 7 May 2019 (UMIN Clinical Trials Registry number: UMIN000036667).

Peer Review reports

Background

Cognitive function, such as executive function, declines with advancing age [1,2,3,4,5,6]. Age-related decline in cognitive function is closely associated with structural changes in the neural substrates [7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15], such as decreased volume in the lateral prefrontal cortex [16,17,18,19,20,21]. To prevent or delay age-related changes in cognitive function, cognitive intervention methods that are based on social activity, such as conversations, have been proposed in some intervention studies [22]. Interestingly, there is a consistent behavioral finding from previous studies that verbal fluency task scores, reflecting executive control and verbal abilities [23], are enhanced by conversation-based interventions in healthy older adults [24, 25]. However, little is known about the neural substrates that underlie the beneficial effect of conversation-based interventions on cognitive function. This pilot study aimed to provide candidate brain regions associated with the enhancement of cognitive function, by collecting structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data from participants in our previous intervention study [25] and comparing the regional brain volume between the intervention and control groups.

Photo-Integrated Conversation Moderated by Robots (PICMOR) is a group conversation-based intervention program that we have developed to train cognitive function in older adults [25]. The group conversation in PICMOR is characterized by strict time management and automatic turn-taking, which are enabled by a robot that acts as a chair. The conversation begins with a talking period, during which the person who is selected as a speaker of topics is prompted by the robot to talk about their daily life for a certain length of time, using photos they have prepared beforehand. The talking period is followed by a discussion period, during which the speaker of topics has to answer questions from the other members of the group. The group members, who are not assigned as the speaker, are required to listen attentively during the speaker’s talks and to ask questions during the discussion period. The robot has been designed to monitor the conversation in real-time and directly prompt and stop the participants’ utterances to balance the production of speech for each participant. The robot moderator enables us to force the participants to make a speech for a certain length of time and finish their talk when they have talked enough. We assumed that executive functions, such as flexibility, planning, working memory, and response inhibition, would be exercised by repeated training in group conversations in PICMOR because the participants are required to make a speech within a limited time, flexibly ask and answer questions, keep in mind and manipulate the information to ask questions, and refrain from interrupting other members of the group in the conversations. To examine the effect of PICMOR on cognitive function in healthy older adults, we previously conducted a randomized controlled trial (RCT) [25]. In the RCT, those who were assigned to the intervention group joined the PICMOR once a week for 12 weeks, whereas those who were assigned to the control group took part in a free conversation program, where unstructured conversations without robotic support were offered. Confirming our assumptions, a beneficial intervention effect was observed in the score of the phonemic verbal fluency task [26, 27], which is often used to measure executive control and verbal abilities [23]. Specifically, a significantly larger increase in the verbal fluency task scores throughout the intervention period was found in the intervention group than in the control group [25]. Based on this finding, we hypothesized that the behavioral effect would be reflected in differences in the brain network responsible for verbal fluency between the two groups that emerged after the intervention period.

To examine the possible difference in the functional network involved in verbal fluency, we conducted an additional experiment for the participants in our previous RCT, using resting-state functional MRI [28, 29]. Indeed, we found significant differences in the resting-state functional connectivity between the two groups, including a significantly higher resting-state functional connectivity between the left and right prefrontal cortex in the intervention group than in the control group [30]. Taken together with evidence from previous neuroimaging and neuropsychological studies that the prefrontal cortex plays an important role in executive functions [31–39] and that the volume of this region has been shown to increase in healthy older adults by an intervention program designed to train executive functions [40], we further hypothesized that this region would have a greater volume in the intervention group than in the control group.

Therefore, in the present study, we applied a voxel-based morphometric (VBM) analysis [41] to the structural MRI data which were obtained after the intervention [30], and examined a possible difference in the regional brain volume between the two groups. In the analysis, we explored the structural difference at the whole-brain level in order to examine the possibility that it could be observed in brain regions other than regions where group difference in resting-state functional connectivity was found, such as the lateral prefrontal cortex [30]. Although we cannot fully attribute any possible differences to the effects of PICMOR because of a lack of comparisons with MRI data from the pre-intervention period, we aimed to identify candidate brain regions that reflect the beneficial effect of conversation-based interventions on brain structures for future research.

Methods

Participants

We re-used the MRI data from our previous study [30]. The data were collected from 31 participants in the intervention group (15 women and 16 men; 20 people with education for 13 years and more; age, mean ± standard deviation [SD] = 72.84 ± 3.45 years) and 30 participants in the control group (17 women and 13 men; 17 people with education for 13 years and more; age, mean ± SD = 72.03 ± 2.72 years) who had taken part in our previous RCT [25]. As reported in our previous study [30], we found no significant difference in age, sex, and education level between the two groups. MRI data were not available for four RCT participants: one participant each in the intervention and control groups had claustrophobia, one participant in the control group had a heart pacemaker, and one participant in the control group declined to undergo MRI scanning [30].

Intervention procedures

The intervention procedures are detailed in our previous studies [25, 30] and briefly described below. In the RCT, 72 community-living older adults were recruited from the Silver Human Resources Center and required to receive screening and baseline assessment by medical interviews, cognitive tests, and self-reported questionnaires. The following exclusion criteria were employed for subsequent intervention phases: dementia; neurological impairment; any disease or medication known to affect the central nervous system; scoring less than 24 in the Japanese version of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE-J) [42]. After the baseline assessment, 65 participants, who were considered as eligible for the RCT, were randomly allocated to the intervention (n = 32) or control (n = 33) groups. After the intervention period, a post-assessment by cognitive tests was conducted for the participants. In addition, MRI data were acquired from 31 and 30 participants in the intervention and control group, respectively, as mentioned above.

During the intervention period, both the intervention and control groups took part in a group conversation once a week for 12 weeks. For the group conversation, 8 subgroups, each with 4 members, were formed in each experimental group (only 1 subgroup in the control group had 5 members). Participants in the intervention group joined the group conversation offered by PICMOR, which is characterized by robotic support (explained in detail below), whereas participants in the control group gathered to talk freely without robotic facilitation among the group members, as they would converse in their daily life.

In the group conversations offered by PICMOR, a robot played the role of a chairperson and encouraged one participant to make a speech within a limited time (1 min) about an event that they had experienced in daily life, using a photo they had prepared beforehand. The photo was displayed on the screen so that it was visible for all group members. The participant selected as a speaker of topics was asked to provide the other members of the group with a total of two events, using two photos (i.e., each speaker was given a total of 2 min to talk). During the 1-min talking period for each event, the other members had to listen carefully to ask questions in the following discussion period. After that, another member was assigned as the speaker of topics. This procedure was repeated until all members finished talking about their topics. The talking period was followed by a 2-min discussion period for each event, during which the speaker of topics was required to answer questions asked by the other members of the group (i.e., each speaker was given a total of 4 min in the discussion). This process was also repeated until all members finished discussing their experiences. In this period, the robot moderated the conversation in real-time by prompting and stopping the participants’ utterances to balance the amount of speech time for each participant. For instance, when the robot detected that one participant had spent less time talking than the other members of the group, it directly encouraged the participant to ask questions or give comments. The robot moderator enabled strict time management and automatic turn-taking based on the actual talking time of each person, which would be difficult for a human moderator.

As noted earlier, we hypothesized that executive functions, including flexibility, planning, working memory, and response inhibition, would be trained by the group conversations in PICMOR, compared to unstructured conversations in the control program, because the participants had to make a speech within a certain length of time (i.e., 1 min), flexibly ask and answer questions, keep in mind and manipulate the information to ask questions, and refrain from interrupting other members of the group. Consistent with this idea, a beneficial intervention effect was observed in the score of the phonemic verbal fluency task [25], in which participants were required to produce as many words as possible beginning with a specific letter within a limited time (1 min) [26, 27]. This behavioral result would be reasonable, given that successful performance of this task requires them to flexibly retrieve appropriate words, keep previously produced words in working memory, and suppress inappropriate words or task-irrelevant thoughts. Behavioral results of multiple cognitive and mental health tests, including the verbal fluency task [30], in the participants of this VBM study (i.e., 31 and 30 participants in the intervention and control group, respectively) are summarized in Additional file 1.

MRI data acquisition and analysis

The procedure of MRI data acquisition is described in our previous study [30]. In short, a high-resolution T1-weighted image was scanned with the following parameters: repetition time = 6.41 ms, echo time = 3.00 ms, field of view = 24.0 cm × 24.0 cm, matrix size = 256 × 256, slice thickness/gap = 1.2/0 mm, and 170 sagittal slices. During the scanning, participants’ head motion was minimized by a belt and foam pads. MRI data were collected using a 3 T Philips Achieva MRI scanner in Advanced Imaging Center Yaesu Clinic, Tokyo. As previously reported in our study [30], we found no significant difference in the period from the last day of interventions to the day of MRI data collection between the intervention (mean ± SD = 9.67 ± 0.76 weeks) and control (mean ± SD = 9.68 ± 0.56 weeks) groups.

The structural MRI data were analyzed using Statistical Parametric Mapping 12 (SPM 12) (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) implemented in MATLAB. In the preprocessing of structural images, five steps were performed. First, the images were segmented into intracranial parts of gray matter (GM), white matter (WM), cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), and other non-brain structures, such as cranium and extracranial tissues, by the standard unified segmentation module [43]. Second, the registration process was implemented by the Diffeomorphic Anatomical Registration using Exponentiated Lie algebra (DARTEL) toolbox [44], which enabled us to achieve a higher accuracy of inter-subject registration. This process began with an import of the DARTEL form of individual segmented GM and WM images, and resulted in the generation of study-specific template images and flow fields data. Third, using this information, we spatially normalized individual segmented images into the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space with a resolution of 1.5 × 1.5 × 1.5 mm3 voxels. Fourth, normalized images were modulated to ensure that relative GM and WM volumes were well preserved after spatial normalization. Finally, these images were smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm full-width at half-maximum.

For statistical analyses of structural MRI data, a binarized mask image was made from the preprocessed images by the masking toolbox (www0.cs.ucl.ac.uk/staff/g.ridgway/masking/) [45]. Employing this mask image as an explicit mask, we conducted a two-sample t-test to identify group differences in regional brain volumes. In the unpaired t-test, the variances of data were assumed to be unequal between the two groups. Moreover, we performed a regression analysis using the difference in verbal fluency task scores between the pre- and post-intervention periods (i.e., post-minus pre-intervention) as the regressor. These models included participants’ age, sex, and education level (≥ 13 years or < 13 years) as nuisance covariates. Total intracranial volumes (GM + WM + CSF) were also entered into the models for global calculation, which enabled us to exclude global nuisance effects (see Additional file 2). In the present study, 1000-iteration Monte-Carlo simulations were employed to determine a sufficient voxel contiguity threshold [46], assuming a type I error voxel-level threshold of p = 0.001 and a cluster extent threshold of p < 0.05. Results indicated a cluster extent of 69 contiguous resampled voxels was sufficient to correct for multiple comparisons. Based on the simulations, clusters with at least 69 voxels were reported in this study. The anatomical sites of the reported regions in the present study were determined with reference to the SPM Anatomy Toolbox [47,48,49] and MRIcro (www.cabi.gatech.edu/mricro/mricro/).

Results

In the present study, the regional brain volume was compared between the intervention and control groups by a two-sample t-test. Consistent with our prediction, the right superior frontal gyrus, i.e., the lateral prefrontal cortex, showed a significantly greater volume in the intervention group than in the control group (Fig. 1). Other regions showing the same pattern were identified in the right parahippocampal gyrus/hippocampus, left posterior middle temporal gyrus, and right postcentral gyrus. In contrast, there were no regions showing significantly greater volume in the control group than in the intervention group. These findings are summarized in Table 1. Results from the regression analysis revealed that the volume of the right inferior parietal lobule was positively correlated with the increased verbal fluency task scores throughout the intervention period (x = 45, y = − 53, z = 51, BA 40, 78 voxels, Z value = 3.67).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Regions showing significantly greater volume in the intervention group than in the control group. Notes: Error bars represent standard errors. Abbreviations: L, left; R, right

Table 1 Regions showing a significant difference in volume between the intervention and control groups

Discussion

This pilot study aimed to provide candidate brain regions that reflect the beneficial effect of conversation-based interventions on cognitive function in healthy older adults, by elucidating a possible difference in regional brain volumes between the intervention and control groups in our previous RCT [25]. The main finding was that the volume of the lateral prefrontal cortex in the intervention group was significantly greater than that in the control group. We also found that the volume of the medial temporal lobe, posterior middle temporal gyrus, and somatosensory cortex showed the same pattern. In contrast, no regions showed significantly greater volume in the control group than in the intervention group. Additionally, the volume of the inferior parietal lobule showed a positive correlation with the increased verbal fluency task scores throughout the intervention period. The present findings imply a possible intervention effect of PICMOR on brain structures. The findings are discussed in detail below.

Our main finding of a significantly greater volume in the lateral prefrontal cortex in the intervention group is consistent with evidence from a previous intervention study, which found that the volume of this region increased in healthy older adults who participated in a music training program designed to train executive functions [40]. Available evidence from neuropsychological studies of patients with brain lesions and neuroimaging studies of healthy adults supports the idea that the prefrontal cortex is involved in executive functions, such as flexibility, planning, working memory, and response inhibition [31–39]. Given that these executive functions would be exercised in the group conversations offered by PICMOR, in which participants have to make a speech within a limited time, flexibly ask and answer questions, intentionally store and manipulate the information to ask questions, and inhibit their interruption of other members [25, 30], it is reasonable that a significant group difference in volume was identified in this region. Taken together with our previous evidence that the resting-state functional connectivity between the left and right prefrontal cortex was significantly higher in the intervention group than in the control group [30], the present finding extends the previous finding by demonstrating that the group difference in the brain region responsible for executive functions emerged at the structural level. Significance of the current VBM finding of the lateral prefrontal cortex could be further supported by an additional analysis for the behavioral measure of multiple cognitive tests before and after the intervention period, in which significant difference in the verbal fluency scores between the two groups was found after the intervention, but not before the intervention (see Additional file 1) [30]. Given that the participants were randomly assigned to the intervention or control group before the intervention [25], and that there was no significant group difference in the verbal fluency scores before the intervention, it is unlikely that the present finding of the lateral prefrontal cortex reflects an intrinsic difference in the volume of this region between the two groups. From these reasons, it would be reasonable to suppose that the lateral prefrontal cortex is one of the candidate brain regions that are responsible for the enhancement of cognitive function by conversation-based interventions.

In this study, the volume of the inferior parietal lobule showed a positive correlation with the increase in the verbal fluency task scores throughout the intervention period. This finding is consistent with those of previous neuroimaging studies showing the contributions of the parietal region to switching during verbal fluency tasks [50,51,52]. Switching is a strategy for successful performance in verbal fluency tasks, which involves consecutively generating words not belonging to the same category [53]. This strategy requires effortful search processes, including cognitive flexibility [53], and has been associated with increased activation in the parietal lobe [50,51,52]. Taken together, the present finding that individuals with higher performance gains in the verbal fluency task had greater volume in the parietal region could reflect individual differences in the switching ability improved by interventions.

The volume of the medial temporal lobe has been shown to increase or be maintained in healthy older adults, who participated in other intervention programs designed to improve memory functions [40, 54, 55]. For example, one intervention study reported that healthy older adults, who joined in a memory training program, showed significantly greater increases in hippocampal volume than those who did not receive such training and patients with subjective memory impairments who received the same training [54]. In another study, both of younger and older adults, who participated in a spatial navigation training program, showed stable hippocampal volumes throughout the intervention period, whereas those who joined in a control program exhibited decreases in the volume [55]. Similar to previous intervention programs, the PICMOR was designed to exercise medial temporal lobe-dependent memory functions during the intervention period. In our previous RCT, participants in the intervention group were repeatedly trained by robotic support to talk about their daily life for a certain length of time, while those in the control group did not receive such training [25]. Talking about one’s own experiences undoubtedly requires the retrieval of autobiographical memories. Also, participants in the intervention group were trained to listen carefully to others and to ask them questions [25]. These aspects should involve the encoding of novel information from others and integration of this information. Given the difference in training demands between the two groups, it might be reasonable that a significant group difference was observed in the medial temporal lobe, as this region plays an important role in retrieval and encoding of episodic memories [56,57,58,59]. However, the present VBM finding of the medial temporal lobe could be limited by behavioral evidence that there was a significant group difference in the scores of the logical memory subtests (Logical memory I & II) from the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R) [60] before the intervention period (see Additional file 1). This behavioral result suggests that the group difference in the volume of this region had already existed even before the intervention period. Further investigation is needed to examine whether the intervention effects could be reflected in the volume of the medial temporal lobe.

The left posterior middle temporal gyrus is included in a classical language-related region, termed Wernicke’s area [61]. Previous neuropsychological studies have consistently reported that patients with lesions in this region show impairment in language comprehension [62,63,64]. Although the precise functional contribution of the posterior middle temporal gyrus to the understanding of spoken language is still a matter of debate [65], one possible explanation by the Dual Stream Model of speech perception is that this region plays a pivotal role in language comprehension as a lexical interface, which matches the phonological structures of perceived words with the corresponding semantic structures [66,67,68]. As noted above, participants in the intervention group were trained to listen carefully to other members of the group and to ask them questions in the group conversations [25]. This aspect of training requires participants to comprehend the conceptual content of others’ utterances. From the perspective of the Dual Stream Model [66,67,68], the present VBM finding of the left posterior middle temporal gyrus could be interpreted as evidence of an enhanced or maintained ability of the intervention group to map phonological structures of utterances by the other members of the group onto their semantic structures during spoken language comprehension.

In this study, regions whose volume was different between the two groups were identified in several areas. However, almost all regions were not identical to the regions where the group difference in resting-state functional connectivity was observed [30]. This discrepancy could be derived from the difference in analytical approaches adopted in the current VBM study and the previous resting-state functional MRI study [30]. The previous study employed a seed-based approach [28, 29], which focuses on resting-state functional connectivity with specific brain areas as seed regions (in this case, the seeds were defined as spheres with a 5-mm radius around specific coordinates located in the left inferior and middle frontal gyri) [30] to characterize the functional network involved in verbal fluency, whereas this study adopted an unbiased approach by exploring group difference in brain volume at the whole-brain level to examine the possibility that the difference could be identified in brain regions other than the regions where group difference in resting-state functional connectivity was observed. Although comprehensive interpretations for the findings from both studies are limited due to this discrepancy, it is noteworthy that both studies supported the significance of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Taken together with ample evidence regarding the contributions of the lateral prefrontal cortex to executive functions [31–39], this region could be modulated in both structural and functional levels by interventions designed to train executive functions.

There are two limitations in this study. One is a lack of comparable MRI data from the pre-intervention period. Although we found a significant group difference in regional brain volumes, we could not determine whether this difference reflects a relatively larger increase in the intervention group than in the control group or a smaller decrease in the intervention group than in the control group, throughout the intervention period, due to the lack of MRI data from the pre-intervention period. Taken together with available evidence showing age-related volume reductions in some regions, including the lateral prefrontal cortex and medial temporal lobe [18, 20, 69,70,71], the latter might be more possible. To address this issue, future research will need to acquire longitudinal data through the intervention period and make comparisons with the data from both groups. The best way to examine this possibility would be performing a two-way mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) with a between-subjects factor of group (intervention, control) and a within-subjects factor of time (pre-intervention, post-intervention) and investigating whether the regional brain volume would show an interaction between the two factors. Despite the limitation, this pilot study achieved its purpose by identifying candidate brain regions that are responsible for the beneficial effect of conversation-based interventions on brain structures.

The other limitation is that some of the reported regions did not meet the significance criteria when additional nuisance covariates were included in the two-sample t-test. Specifically, when the scores of the Japanese version of the 15-item Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS-15-J) [72] after the intervention were employed as an additional nuisance covariate to adjust participants’ mental status, the lateral prefrontal cortex and medial temporal lobe did not achieve statistical significance, whereas the other regions, including the posterior middle temporal gyrus and the somatosensory cortex, remained significant. When the scores of the Japanese version of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA-J) [26] were used to adjust participants’ global cognition, the posterior middle temporal gyrus was significant, while the other regions were not. However, when the MMSE-J scores [42] were employed, all of the reported regions met the significance criteria. Thus, note that a part of the present findings could be limited by some factors, such as mental status and/or global cognition.

The current findings imply structural changes in the brain induced by balanced conversations, in which both inputs and outputs of information are required. Practicing conversations in such a balanced manner in daily life could be an effective approach to maintain or improve cognitive and brain health, and the effectiveness may improve by introducing the proposed robotic system. Our findings support existing policies to increase opportunities for community-dwelling older adults to interact with others. However, an alternative method which has similar effects on cognitive and brain health would have special needs for practice, since some people are reluctant to meet others. Toward addressing this issue, a dialogue-based robot system for daily cognitive training without meeting other people at home has been recently developed [73]. The user of the dialogue-based robot system listens carefully to the robotic speech and asks questions based on the speech, which requires both inputs and outputs of information. As the next step, the effects of this dialogue system on cognitive health in older adults would be examined in future research.

Conclusions

In this pilot study, we examined the difference in brain structures between the intervention and control groups after the intervention. Our results demonstrated that the volume of the superior frontal gyrus was greater in the intervention group than in the control group. Greater volume in the intervention group was also identified in the parahippocampal gyrus/hippocampus, posterior middle temporal gyrus, and postcentral gyrus. In contrast, no regions showed greater volume in the control group than in the intervention group. Furthermore, the region whose volume was positively correlated with the increase in verbal fluency scores was found in the inferior parietal lobule. This pilot study successfully identified candidate brain regions that reflect the beneficial effect of conversation-based interventions on cognitive function, including the lateral prefrontal cortex, which plays a pivotal role in executive functions. Further investigation will be needed to confirm this possible effect by collecting and comparing MRI data from the pre- and post-intervention periods in future studies.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and/or analyzed in the present study are not publicly available due to the requirement of a joint research agreement for data sharing, but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

MRI:

Magnetic resonance imaging

PICMOR:

Photo-Integrated Conversation Moderated by Robots

RCT:

Randomized controlled trial

VBM:

Voxel-based morphometry

SD:

Standard deviation

MMSE-J:

Japanese version of the Mini-Mental State Examination

SPM:

Statistical Parametric Mapping

GM:

Gray matter

WM:

White matter

CSF:

Cerebrospinal fluid

DARTEL:

Diffeomorphic Anatomical Registration using Exponentiated Lie algebra

MNI:

Montreal Neurological Institute

WMS-R:

Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised

ANOVA:

Analysis of variance

GDS-15-J:

Japanese version of the 15-item Geriatric Depression Scale

MoCA-J:

Japanese version of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment

References

  1. Nyberg L, Pudas S. Successful memory aging. Annu Rev Psychol. 2019;70:219–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Park DC, Bischof GN. The aging mind: neuroplasticity in response to cognitive training. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2013;15(1):109–19.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Park DC, Lautenschlager G, Hedden T, Davidson NS, Smith AD, Smith PK. Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychol Aging. 2002;17(2):299–320.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Salthouse T. Consequences of age-related cognitive declines. Annu Rev Psychol. 2012;63:201–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Salthouse TA. Selective review of cognitive aging. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2010;16(5):754–60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Salthouse TA. Trajectories of normal cognitive aging. Psychol Aging. 2019;34(1):17–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Antonenko D, Floel A. Healthy aging by staying selectively connected: a mini-review. Gerontology. 2014;60(1):3–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Goh JO, Park DC. Neuroplasticity and cognitive aging: the scaffolding theory of aging and cognition. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2009;27(5):391–403.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Grady C. The cognitive neuroscience of ageing. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13(7):491–505.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Greenwood PM. Functional plasticity in cognitive aging: review and hypothesis. Neuropsychology. 2007;21(6):657–73.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Hedden T, Gabrieli JD. Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5(2):87–96.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Nyberg L, Lovden M, Riklund K, Lindenberger U, Backman L. Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(5):292–305.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Park DC, Reuter-Lorenz P. The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009;60:173–96.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Reuter-Lorenz PA, Park DC. Human neuroscience and the aging mind: a new look at old problems. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2010;65(4):405–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Salthouse TA. Neuroanatomical substrates of age-related cognitive decline. Psychol Bull. 2011;137(5):753–84.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Lemaitre H, Goldman AL, Sambataro F, Verchinski BA, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Weinberger DR, et al. Normal age-related brain morphometric changes: nonuniformity across cortical thickness, surface area and gray matter volume? Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33(3):617 e1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Manard M, Bahri MA, Salmon E, Collette F. Relationship between grey matter integrity and executive abilities in aging. Brain Res. 2016;1642:562–80.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Nyberg L, Salami A, Andersson M, Eriksson J, Kalpouzos G, Kauppi K, et al. Longitudinal evidence for diminished frontal cortex function in aging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107(52):22682–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Ramanoel S, Hoyau E, Kauffmann L, Renard F, Pichat C, Boudiaf N, et al. Gray matter volume and cognitive performance during normal aging. A voxel-based morphometry study. Front Aging Neurosci. 2018;10:235.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Raz N, Lindenberger U, Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM, Head D, Williamson A, et al. Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: general trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15(11):1676–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Tisserand DJ, van Boxtel MP, Pruessner JC, Hofman P, Evans AC, Jolles J. A voxel-based morphometric study to determine individual differences in gray matter density associated with age and cognitive change over time. Cereb Cortex. 2004;14(9):966–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Kelly ME, Duff H, Kelly S, McHugh Power JE, Brennan S, Lawlor BA, et al. The impact of social activities, social networks, social support and social relationships on the cognitive functioning of healthy older adults: a systematic review. Syst Rev. 2017;6(1):259.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Shao Z, Janse E, Visser K, Meyer AS. What do verbal fluency tasks measure? Predictors of verbal fluency performance in older adults. Front Psychol. 2014;5:772.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Dodge HH, Zhu J, Mattek N, Bowman M, Ybarra O, Wild K, et al. Web-enabled conversational interactions as a means to improve cognitive functions: results of a 6-week randomized controlled trial. Alzheimers Dement (N Y). 2015;1(1):1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Otake-Matsuura M, Tokunaga S, Watanabe K, Abe MS, Sekiguchi T, Sugimoto H, et al. Cognitive intervention through Photo-Integrated Conversation Moderated by Robots (PICMOR) program: a randomized controlled trial. Front Robot AI. 2021;8:633076.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Fujiwara Y, Suzuki H, Yasunaga M, Sugiyama M, Ijuin M, Sakuma N, et al. Brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment in older Japanese: validation of the Japanese version of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2010;10(3):225–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Bigler ED, Tranel D. Neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford University Press; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Miro-Padilla A, Bueicheku E, Ventura-Campos N, Palomar-Garcia MA, Avila C. Functional connectivity in resting state as a phonemic fluency ability measure. Neuropsychologia. 2017;97:98–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Sala-Llonch R, Bartres-Faz D, Junque C. Reorganization of brain networks in aging: a review of functional connectivity studies. Front Psychol. 2015;6:663.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Sugimoto H, Kawagoe T, Otake-Matsuura M. Characteristics of resting-state functional connectivity in older adults after the PICMOR intervention program: a preliminary report. BMC Geriatr. 2020;20(1):486.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Alvarez JA, Emory E. Executive function and the frontal lobes: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychol Rev. 2006;16(1):17–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Blumenfeld RS, Ranganath C. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex promotes long-term memory formation through its role in working memory organization. J Neurosci. 2006;26(3):916–25.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Buchsbaum BR, Greer S, Chang WL, Berman KF. Meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of the Wisconsin Card-Sorting task and component processes. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25(1):35–45.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Friedman NP, Robbins TW. The role of prefrontal cortex in cognitive control and executive function. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2022;47(1):72–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Laird AR, McMillan KM, Lancaster JL, Kochunov P, Turkeltaub PE, Pardo JV, et al. A comparison of label-based review and ALE meta-analysis in the Stroop task. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25(1):6–21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Owen AM, McMillan KM, Laird AR, Bullmore E. N-back working memory paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25(1):46–59.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Steele VR, Aharoni E, Munro GE, Calhoun VD, Nyalakanti P, Stevens MC, et al. A large scale (N=102) functional neuroimaging study of response inhibition in a Go/NoGo task. Behav Brain Res. 2013;256:529–36.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Varjacic A, Mantini D, Demeyere N, Gillebert CR. Neural signatures of Trail Making Test performance: evidence from lesion-mapping and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia. 2018;115:78–87.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Yuan P, Raz N. Prefrontal cortex and executive functions in healthy adults: a meta-analysis of structural neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;42:180–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. West GL, Zendel BR, Konishi K, Benady-Chorney J, Bohbot VD, Peretz I, et al. Playing Super Mario 64 increases hippocampal grey matter in older adults. PLoS One. 2017;12(12):e0187779.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Voxel-based morphometry--the methods. Neuroimage. 2000;11(6 Pt 1):805–21.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Sugishita M, Koshizuka Y, Sudou S, Sugishita K, Hemmi I, Karasawa H, et al. The validity and reliability of the Japanese version of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE-J) with the original procedure of the Attention and Calculation Task (2001). Japan J Cogn Neurosci. 2018;20(2):91–110.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Unified segmentation. Neuroimage. 2005;26(3):839–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Ashburner J. A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage. 2007;38(1):95–113.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Ridgway GR, Omar R, Ourselin S, Hill DL, Warren JD, Fox NC. Issues with threshold masking in voxel-based morphometry of atrophied brains. Neuroimage. 2009;44(1):99–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Slotnick SD. Cluster success: fMRI inferences for spatial extent have acceptable false-positive rates. Cogn Neurosci. 2017;8(3):150–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Eickhoff SB, Heim S, Zilles K, Amunts K. Testing anatomically specified hypotheses in functional imaging using cytoarchitectonic maps. Neuroimage. 2006;32(2):570–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Eickhoff SB, Paus T, Caspers S, Grosbras MH, Evans AC, Zilles K, et al. Assignment of functional activations to probabilistic cytoarchitectonic areas revisited. Neuroimage. 2007;36(3):511–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Eickhoff SB, Stephan KE, Mohlberg H, Grefkes C, Fink GR, Amunts K, et al. A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. Neuroimage. 2005;25(4):1325–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Gurd JM, Amunts K, Weiss PH, Zafiris O, Zilles K, Marshall JC, et al. Posterior parietal cortex is implicated in continuous switching between verbal fluency tasks: an fMRI study with clinical implications. Brain. 2002;125(Pt 5):1024–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Hirshorn EA, Thompson-Schill SL. Role of the left inferior frontal gyrus in covert word retrieval: neural correlates of switching during verbal fluency. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(12):2547–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Scheuringer A, Harris TA, Pletzer B. Recruiting the right hemisphere: sex differences in inter-hemispheric communication during semantic verbal fluency. Brain Lang. 2020;207:104814.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Troyer AK, Moscovitch M, Winocur G. Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: evidence from younger and older healthy adults. Neuropsychology. 1997;11(1):138–46.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Engvig A, Fjell AM, Westlye LT, Skaane NV, Dale AM, Holland D, et al. Effects of cognitive training on gray matter volumes in memory clinic patients with subjective memory impairment. J Alzheimers Dis. 2014;41(3):779–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Lovden M, Schaefer S, Noack H, Bodammer NC, Kuhn S, Heinze HJ, et al. Spatial navigation training protects the hippocampus against age-related changes during early and late adulthood. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33(3):620 e9–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Davachi L. Item, context and relational episodic encoding in humans. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2006;16(6):693–700.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Diana RA, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C. Imaging recollection and familiarity in the medial temporal lobe: a three-component model. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11(9):379–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Eichenbaum H, Sauvage M, Fortin N, Komorowski R, Lipton P. Towards a functional organization of episodic memory in the medial temporal lobe. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36(7):1597–608.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Rugg MD, Vilberg KL, Mattson JT, Yu SS, Johnson JD, Suzuki M. Item memory, context memory and the hippocampus: fMRI evidence. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50(13):3070–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Wechsler D. WMS-R: Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised Manual. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1987.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Ardila A, Bernal B, Rosselli M. How localized are language brain areas? A review of Brodmann areas involvement in oral language. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2016;31(1):112–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Dronkers NF, Wilkins DP, Van Valin RD, Jr., Redfern BB, Jaeger JJ. Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in language comprehension. Cognition. 2004;92(1–2):145–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Kertesz A, Sheppard A, MacKenzie R. Localization in transcortical sensory aphasia. Arch Neurol. 1982;39(8):475–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Robson H, Sage K, Ralph MA. Wernicke’s aphasia reflects a combination of acoustic-phonological and semantic control deficits: a case-series comparison of Wernicke’s aphasia, semantic dementia and semantic aphasia. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50(2):266–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Kemmerer D. Cognitive neuroscience of language. New York: Psychology Press; 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Hickok G, Poeppel D. Towards a functional neuroanatomy of speech perception. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000;4(4):131–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Hickok G, Poeppel D. Dorsal and ventral streams: a framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language. Cognition. 2004;92(1–2):67–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Hickok G, Poeppel D. The cortical organization of speech processing. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8(5):393–402.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Fjell AM, Walhovd KB, Fennema-Notestine C, McEvoy LK, Hagler DJ, Holland D, et al. One-year brain atrophy evident in healthy aging. J Neurosci. 2009;29(48):15223–31.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Gorbach T, Pudas S, Lundquist A, Oradd G, Josefsson M, Salami A, et al. Longitudinal association between hippocampus atrophy and episodic-memory decline. Neurobiol Aging. 2017;51:167–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Leong RLF, Lo JC, Sim SKY, Zheng H, Tandi J, Zhou J, et al. Longitudinal brain structure and cognitive changes over 8 years in an East Asian cohort. Neuroimage. 2017;147:852–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Sugishita K, Sugishita M, Hemmi I, Asada T, Tanigawa T. A validity and reliability study of the Japanese version of the Geriatric Depression Scale 15 (GDS-15-J). Clin Gerontol. 2017;40(4):233–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Tokunaga S, Tamura K, Otake-Matsuura M. A dialogue-based system with photo and storytelling for older adults: toward daily cognitive training. Front Robot AI. 2021;8:644964.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the staff of Advanced Imaging Center Yaesu Clinic for their technical assistance in the MRI scanning. We also thank Drs. Takuya Sekiguchi and Seiki Tokunaga for their support to improve the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant Numbers JP18KT0035 (MO-M), JP19H01138 (MO-M), JP20H05022 (MO-M), JP20H05574 (MO-M), and JP19K14489 (HS). The funder had no role in the design of the study and collection, analysis, and interpretation of data and in writing the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

MO-M designed this study. HS collected and analyzed the data and wrote the manuscript under the supervision of MO-M. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hikaru Sugimoto.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All participants provided their written informed consent to the protocol approved by the Institutional Review Board of RIKEN (Wako3 30–11 (2)). All methods were performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1. 

Behavioral results of pre/post cognitive and mental health tests in the intervention (n = 31) and control (n = 30) groups.

Additional file 2. 

VBM outputs in the intervention (n = 31) and control (n = 30) groups.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sugimoto, H., Otake-Matsuura, M. A pilot voxel-based morphometry study of older adults after the PICMOR intervention program. BMC Geriatr 22, 63 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-021-02669-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-021-02669-x

Keywords